Abstract
Free full text
Temporal Specificity of Perceptual Learning in an Auditory Discrimination Task
Abstract
Although temporal processing is used in a wide range of sensory and motor tasks, there is little evidence as to whether a single centralized clock or a distributed system underlies timing in the range of tens to hundreds of milliseconds. We investigated this question by studying whether learning on an auditory interval discrimination task generalizes across stimulus types, intervals, and frequencies. The degree to which improvements in timing carry over to different stimulus features constrains the neural mechanisms underlying timing. Human subjects trained on a 100- or 200-msec interval discrimination task showed an improvement in temporal resolution. This learning generalized to a perceptually distinct duration stimulus, as well as to the trained interval presented with tones at untrained spectral frequencies. The improvement in performance did not generalize to untrained intervals. To determine if spectral generalization was dependent on the importance of frequency information in the task, subjects were simultaneously trained on two different intervals identified by frequency. As a whole, our results indicate that the brain uses circuits that are dedicated to specific time spans, and that each circuit processes stimuli across nontemporal stimulus features. The patterns of generalization additionally indicate that temporal learning does not rely on changes in early, subcortical processing, because the nontemporal features are encoded by different channels at early stages.
The auditory system is capable of temporal processing across a wide range of scales from microsecond timing, used in sound localization, up to several seconds (Jeffress 1948; Carr 1993; Buonomano and Karmarkar 2002). Timing in the range of tens to hundreds of milliseconds is a fundamental part of a wide range of sensory and motor tasks (Ivry 1996; Gibbon et al. 1997; Buonomano and Karmarkar 2002). In the auditory system, this range is used in the ability to discriminate the order, interval, and duration of sounds, which is important for speech processing (Shannon et al. 1995; Doupe and Kuhl 1999). Indeed, it is thought that deficits in this range of temporal processing may contribute to some language-based learning disabilities (Merzenich et al. 1996; Tallal et al. 1996). However, even for simple interval discrimination tasks, little is known about the neural mechanisms and areas involved in timing.
One unanswered question regarding temporal processing relates to the degree of its specificity for nontemporal features. For example, if a tone sounds at a regular interval, would the brain measure the interval with the same circuitry if the pitch of the tone was different? The answer would be yes if timing is centralized, meaning that temporal processing across qualitatively different tasks within or even across modalities is all computed by the same neural mechanisms. Alternatively, timing could be distributed, meaning that each perceptually different task requires its own circuitry. A second type of specificity concerns the task's temporal features themselves. A single ‘universal clock’ could be the sole timing mechanism for all intervals and/or durations, or there could be a set of dedicated circuits, each specific to given lengths of time (also called interval-based mechanisms; Ivry 1996).
It is possible to address these issues by examining the specificity of perceptual learning on an auditory temporal discrimination task. Previous studies have shown that auditory interval discrimination tasks undergo perceptual learning after training (Wright et al. 1997; Nagarajan et al. 1998; Westheimer 1999). Typically, the stimuli in these experiments consist of two brief tones separated by either a standard interval (T) or a longer comparison interval equivalent to the standard plus a value Δt. The subject is presented with both the standard and comparison interval and asked to judge whether the longer interval occurred first or second. Training over several days on a particular interval leads to improved performance as measured by a decreased, more accurate discrimination threshold. These experiments have shown that learning generalizes across untrained frequencies (the spatial domain) but not across intervals (the temporal domain; Wright et al. 1997). Other studies have indicated that improvements in interval discrimination generalize across sensory modalities (Nagarajan et al. 1998; Westheimer 1999), or even from a sensory to a motor task (Meegan et al. 2000).
The learning observed in the work described earlier could result from improved timing per se, or an enhanced ability to store and/or compare the standard and comparison stimuli. In the present study, experiments are designed to discriminate between these two hypotheses by using a single stimulus protocol in which the subjects received only a test interval rather than both a test and the standard interval on each trial (see Materials and Methods). Subjects were trained in this manner on one interval and tested to see if the resultant learning generalized to the trained stimulus presented with alternate tone pitches. Additionally, generalization to a perceptually distinct continuous tone with the same duration of the trained interval was also examined. The goal of this design was to increase the subjects' focus on the time span represented by the stimulus and decrease the use of their ability to store and compare stimuli (see Discussion).
Experiments on auditory temporal processing generally train on one particular target stimulus. However, experiments in the visual system have shown that the type of task being performed during training can determine the features over which learning generalizes (Ahissar and Hochstein 1997). Thus the generalization patterns for interval learning may reflect the ease or difficulty of the task. To examine this possibility, naive subjects performed a parallel learning experiment in which they were trained on two intervals simultaneously. In addition to the increased difficulty because of a higher memory load, subjects were forced to attend to the frequency of the stimulus' tones, as it signaled which of the two standards to use for comparison. However, subjects showed significant learning and generalization across frequencies on both trained intervals. Together, the results from our experiments provide evidence for centralized but dedicated, or interval-specific, timing mechanisms.
RESULTS
Interval Generalization
We first established that perceptual learning of interval discrimination is observed with the single-stimulus protocol adopted here. Figure Figure1A1A shows the average learning curve for 13 subjects trained for 10 d on the 100-msec interval discrimination task with tones at 1 kHz. An orthogonal trend analysis revealed a significant linear component (F1,120=28.7, P<0.001), indicating that performance improved with training. Because our goal was to examine how perceptual learning generalizes, each subject's learning curve was analyzed independently. All of the individual curves are presented in Figure Figure1B.1B. Ten of the 13 subjects exhibited significant learning curves as determined by orthogonal linear trend analysis (P<0.05).
To examine transfer of learning, we tested subjects on four different conditions before (pretest) and after training (posttest). Three of the conditions were intervals with the following properties: the trained stimulus of 100 msec at 1 kHz, the 100-msec interval at an alternate frequency of 3.75 kHz tones, and an alternate interval of 200 msec at the trained 1-kHz frequency. The fourth condition, 100-msec-dur (100 msec-duration) was based on the trained time span and a 1-kHz frequency but was presented as a single continuous tone so that subjects had to discern changes in tone duration. Subjectively, interval and duration stimuli are perceptually very different. The pretest and posttest data from the 10 subjects that had significant learning curves are shown in Figure Figure2.2. A two-way analysis of variance with repeated measures on one factor (pretest×posttest) revealed a significant effect of training (F1,36=42, P<0.001) as well as a significant interaction (F1,36=5.4, P<0.005). Paired t-tests revealed a significant difference between the pre- and posttest for all of the conditions except the 200-msec/1-kHz interval task. Additionally, there was not a significant difference as determined by F-test for effect of trials between the size of the learning effect between the trained and the 100-msec/3.75-kHz conditions (F1,36=0.27, P<0.61) and between the trained and 100-msec-dur tasks (F1,36=2.86, P=0.10), indicating complete generalization to the 100-msec/3.75-kHz and 100-msec dur tasks.
It is important to determine whether this pattern of generalization is specific to training on 100-msec intervals or is a general feature of interval learning. To verify this issue, we performed a similar set of experiments, except that subjects were trained on an interval of 200 msec with 1-kHz tones. Analogous to the 100-msec generalization experiments, the four test conditions were 200 msec/1 kHz (trained condition), 100 msec/1 kHz, 200 msec/3.75 kHz, and 200-msec dur at 1 kHz. Five of the 12 subjects exhibited significant learning curves, indicating that the 200-msec task was a more difficult learning task. The lower percentage of learners may also be due to a floor effect, as many subjects in this group had low pretest threshold values. When the pre- and posttest values of the learners were examined, the results were qualitatively equivalent to those for the 100-msec-based generalization task. A two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on pretest×posttest showed that training had a significant effect (F1,16=40.32, P<0.001) as well as a significant interaction (F1,16=3.88, P<0.05). As shown in Figure Figure3,3, learners improved significantly on the trained task as well as on the tasks based on the trained interval, but showed no significant learning on the untrained interval. F-tests for differences in learning within groups were not significant between the trained condition and the 200-msec/3.75-kHz task (F1,16=1.32, P=0.26) or for the 200-msec dur condition (F1,16=2.77, P=0.12). This is consistent with the results of the 100-msec-based experiments, showing that learning on the trained condition generalized completely across frequency and stimulus type.
Parallel LearningExperiments
These experiments present evidence that improvements in interval discrimination generalize to a duration discrimination task and to interval stimuli presented at untrained frequencies. It is possible, however, that frequency generalization was observed because the task did not require the subjects to attend to the frequency of the stimuli. To examine this issue, we trained subjects simultaneously on two different target intervals identified by their frequency. Twenty subjects underwent a testing procedure identical to that of the previous experiments with the following four conditions: 50 msec/1 kHz, 50 msec/4 kHz, 200 msec/4 kHz, and 200 msec/1 kHz. However, the organization of the training sessions was slightly different. For each block, subjects heard target stimuli for a 50-msec/1k-Hz interval and then a 200-msec/4 kHz interval. After this, they were presented with 120 trials that randomly interleaved stimuli that were either based on the 50-msec/1-kHz or 200-msec/4-kHz conditions. In this experiment, frequency signaled the target interval on which the subjects needed to base their judgment. For example, if the subject received a 100-msec stimulus presented with 1-kHz tones, the correct response would be “long”, as the lower frequency indicates the subject should base their answer on the 50-msec target. However, “short” would be the correct response if the stimulus used 4-kHz tones, as the correct target would be 200-msec long.
Overall, subjects showed simultaneous learning on both the 50-msec/1-kHz and the 200-msec/4-kHz training intervals. The average learning curves for all 20 subjects are presented in Figure Figure4.4. Both these curves showed a significant orthogonal trend. From the 20 subjects, 11 showed significant learning on both the 50- and 200-msec intervals. There were 6 subjects that showed significant learning on only one of the two intervals (the remaining 3 showed no learning). Five of these 6 improved on the 50-msec interval. Consistent with the 200-msec interval generalization experiments, the 200-msec stimuli may be more difficult to learn. It is also possible that these subjects chose to pay less attention to one type of stimuli when performing the task. As shown in Figure Figure5,5, the pre- and posttest data for the 11 subjects that learned on both intervals revealed a significant effect of training (F1,40=124, P<0.001). However, the interaction was not significant (F1,40=1.31, P=0.28), indicating that there was an improvement in performance across all of the conditions. These learners improved significantly on the trained conditions, but also on the untrained conditions, thus showing complete spectral generalization despite the importance of frequency in this task.
DISCUSSION
The results presented here show that temporal perception, as measured by interval discrimination, undergoes learning. This form of perceptual learning is specific to the time of the trained interval, but generalizes from an interval to duration (filled interval) task, as well as to novel frequencies. The transfer of learning to novel frequencies is consistent with previous reports in which a two-interval, forced-choice paradigm was used (Wright et al. 1997; Nagarajan et al. 1998; Westheimer 1999).
Many experiments have examined perceptual learning in the visual system and whether it generalizes across various stimulus features. These studies have shown high degrees of both task and retinotopic specificity. For example, spatial discrimination learning on pop-out, vernier, and bisection tasks generally does not transfer to different stimuli orientations or to untrained retinal quadrants (Karni and Sagi 1991; Fahle et al. 1995; Crist et al. 1997). These psychophysical data have been of value in helping to narrow down the neural basis of visual learning. They indicate that learning may be the result of changes occurring in V1, because V1 is the first site in which orientation-selective neurons are found, and these cells still have small receptive fields (Gilbert et al. 2001). Adini et al. (2002) have proposed a mechanism for contrast perception learning in visual cortex based on a model of plasticity consistent with their psychophysical data. More directly, a study of V1 after perceptual learning specific to both stimulus orientation and location found a correlated cellular change (Schoups et al. 2000). In this study, plasticity in the tuning of a subgroup of V1 cells related to the trained orientation was proposed to underlie perceptual learning. Another study has failed to observe such electrophysiological changes; however, the behavioral results were also different, as the learning that was seen generalized across locations (Ghose et al. 2002).
In contrast with visual perceptual learning, few data are available on temporal perceptual learning in the auditory system. Specifically, it is not known whether the neural loci for timing are distributed throughout multiple areas or centralized to specific structures (Ivry 1996; Buonomano and Karmarkar 2002). The cerebellum, which contributes to the timing of motor responses (Ivry 1993; Perrett et al. 1993; Medina et al. 2000), has also been implicated in the timing of sensory events (Ivry and Keele 1989; Ivry 1993; Jueptner et al. 1995). Others have presented evidence that timing in the range of hundreds of milliseconds is localized in basal ganglia (Artieda et al. 1992; Harrington et al. 1998a) or cortical areas (Harrington et al. 1998b; Onoe et al. 2001).
Although the current study does not directly address the localization of timing, it, too, can offer insights based on its generalization profiles and the known functional anatomy of the auditory system. However, it is important to establish that the observed learning is actually due to an improvement in temporal resolution. Learning in previous experiments could have been due to an overall enhancement in the ability to store and compare stimuli. Assuming that the focus of this strategy would be the temporal difference between the interval stimuli for comparison, it is compatible with generalization to novel frequencies and not to novel intervals. The current experiments decreased the likelihood of this strategy in two ways. The first was to remove the standard interval from the trials in both testing and training, thus eliminating trial-by-trial comparison of the target and standard interval. The design of this experiment, therefore, encouraged subjects to form a dynamic representation of the time frame involved, and to adjust this timing based on the feedback from their decisions. Explicit memorization of the standard stimulus is not possible during the trials, and subjects reported listening to the standard only one to five times before the trial block. We found that subjects still showed statistically significant improvements in their discrimination thresholds after training with this protocol, indicating that the underlying cause for learning was an improvement in the actual resolution of the mechanisms underlying timing.
The second aspect of our approach was to examine generalization to a duration discrimination task. Perceptually, the duration and interval stimuli are very different. Indeed, previous research has shown that duration and interval discrimination thresholds differ significantly, which implies that they may represent separate tasks (Rammsayer and Lima 1991; Grondin et al. 1998). Thus, it seems likely that improvement in the ability to store or compare stimuli should not generalize to a duration task. Because we observed generalization from the interval to duration task, our results indicate that learning relied on changes in temporal processing.
The focus of this study is on the specificity of timing mechanisms, and that is determined here by the generalization profile of training-dependent learning. It is possible that significant learning is taking place during the pretest; however, this learning may be confounded with improvements because of increased familiarity with the experimental setup. It is also impossible to separate the contribution of any particular stimulus condition to learning in data from the pretest. By using data from subjects that showed significant improvement as a result of training, we can examine the generalization produced specifically by temporal learning on the trained stimulus.
The generalization to novel frequencies, but not to novel intervals, in these experiments indicates that timing for any given interval relies on centralized circuits that are accessed across different channels. However, it is possible that independent timing mechanisms exist for each frequency, but if a task does not require attending to a given frequency, all the circuits can undergo learning through top-down mechanisms. Experiments in the visual system have shown that the type of task being performed during training can determine the features over which learning generalizes. For example, Ahissar and Hochstein (1997) have shown that increasing task difficulty by including a larger training stimulus set can decrease generalization. Thus the generalization seen in interval learning may also reflect the difficulty of the task, or more generally the salient features of the task. The parallel learning experiments presented here indicate that this is not the case. This task was more difficult, as two different intervals were learned simultaneously and it required attention to frequency information. Because generalization was still observed to novel frequencies, these data indicate that this pattern of generalization is a general feature of temporal perceptual learning.
The specificity for spatial location of perceptual learning in the visual system is generally taken as evidence that learning relies on retinotopic changes in V1 (Gilbert et al. 2001). Our observation that temporal perceptual learning in the auditory system generalizes to untrained stimulus frequencies indicates that timing mechanisms may not be located in tonotopically organized structures such as A1, because most reported receptive fields there do not respond to both 1 and 4 kHz (Phillips and Irvine 1981; Recanzone et al. 2000). However, it should be noted that some primary auditory cortex cells, in cats, for example, can have broad, even multipeaked tuning curves (Sutter and Schreiner 1991), making it physiologically possible for A1 neurons to account for frequency generalization.
Auditory learning also transferred from an interval to a duration task based on the same time span. This diverges from visual psychophysics results, in which spatial learning on tasks such as bisection discrimination does not generalize to even a vernier task that involves the same visual stimuli (Crist et al. 1997). Overall, although most visual learning is specific to several visual stimulus features, the perceptual learning in the auditory experiments presented here is specific only to the temporal domain. Because the characteristics of most subcortical auditory neurons as described in animal preparations (Kiang et al. 1965; Godfrey et al. 1975a,b; Nuding et al. 1999) indicate that they would respond differently to unfilled (interval) and filled (duration) stimuli, it is unlikely that temporal learning relies on early subcortical plasticity mechanisms. Rather, our data indicate that learning is occurring at later, less dedicated stages of auditory processing, where interval and duration stimuli have common integration pathways. These sites would include cortical areas and cerebellar circuits receiving cortical input.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Experimental Subjects
A total of 45 volunteers between the ages of 18 and 30 yr with normal hearing participated in one of the three types of experiments described as follows. Subjects performed the experiment at the same time daily (with the exception of weekends) for 14 d. The subjects had no previous experience in this type of discrimination task and were paid for their participation.
Protocol
A single stimulus protocol was used for all experiments. At the beginning of the trial block for a particular condition, subjects were presented with a target stimulus of two tones of a specific frequency separated by a fixed interval (T). Participants could choose to listen to this target as many times as they wished. After this stage, only test stimuli of a single pair of tones that were either shorter or longer than the target stimulus (T±Δt) were presented. Subjects made a forced-choice decision as to whether the trial stimulus seemed shorter or longer than the target interval by pressing one of two buttons on a computer mouse. They were provided with immediate visual feedback. All stimuli were generated in Matlab and presented through Optimus Pro 40 headphones. Tones were 15 msec long including a 5 msec on-and-off ramp.
All experiments consisted of 2 d of pretest, 10 d of a training phase, and 2 d of posttest. For the pre- and posttest, subjects completed the experimental task for four test conditions (see following), which were presented in a random order. Training was done for 10 d on a specified subset of the test conditions.
100-msec and 200-msec Interval GeneralizationExperiments
Independent experiments were run in which subjects were trained on either a 100- or 200-msec target interval. Each session, whether on a testing or training day, consisted of 12 blocks of 60 trials each. On pre- and posttest days, the subjects ran each of the four test conditions a total of three times. For the 100-msec-based tasks, the testing conditions were three types of interval targets: 100 msec/1-kHz tones, 100 msec/3.75 kHz, and 200 msec/1 kHz. The fourth condition was a duration-based task in which the target was a single 1-kHz tone with a 100-msec duration. For the 10 training days, subjects received 12 blocks on the target condition (100 msec/1 kHz).
To determine that results were a general property of interval learning, rather than specific to the 100-msec target interval, a separate group was trained with a 200-msec/1-kHz target stimulus. In these experiments, the test conditions were 100 msec/1 kHz, 200 msec/1 kHz, 200 msec/3.75 kHz, and a 200-msec/1-kHz duration target.
Parallel IntervalLearning
In these experiments, subjects were trained on two stimulus conditions concurrently. During pre- and posttesting the following four interval conditions were used: 50 msec/1 kHz, 50 msec/4 kHz, 200 msec/1 kHz, and 200 msec/4 kHz. These were each tested in blocks of 60 trials twice in random order on the two pretest and two posttest days. Subjects were trained on two intervals simultaneously. Training was done on seven blocks of 120 trials with trial stimuli based on 50-msec/1-kHz and 200-msec/4-kHz targets randomly interleaved within the blocks.
Analysis and Determination ofThreshold
Threshold values were determined in the manner described previously (Wright et al. 1997). In brief, an adaptive procedure was used during each block, in which Δt was decreased after three correct answers, and increased after one incorrect answer. The Δt values at points that reversed from increasing to decreasing or vice versa were recorded. Because the stimuli in our protocols had an interval of T±Δt, we defined threshold as 2 multiplied by the mean of all reversal values excluding the first three. Significance of the learning curves was determined by linear orthogonal trend analysis (Bruning and Kintz 1987).
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Dr. Steven Engel for comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. This research was supported by the EJLB Foundation, the DoD (NDSEG), and by the NSF.
The publication costs of this article were defrayed in part by payment of page charges. This article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement” in accordance with 18 USC section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.
Footnotes
E-MAIL ude.alcu@onoubd; FAX (310) 825-2224.
Article and publication are at http://www.learnmem.org/cgi/doi/10.1101/lm.55503.
REFERENCES
- Adini A, Sagi D, Tsodyks M. Context-enabled learning in the human visual system. Nature. 2002;415:790–793. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Ahissar M, Hochstein S. Task difficulty and the specificity of perceptual learning. Nature. 1997;387:401–406. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Artieda J, Pastor MA, Lacruz F, Obeso JA. Temporal discrimination is abnormal in Parkinson's disease. Brain. 1992;115:199–210. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Bruning JL, Kintz BL. Computational handbook of statistics. 3rd ed. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman and Company; 1987. pp. 145–158. [Google Scholar]
- Buonomano DV, Karmarkar UR. How do we tell time? Neuroscientist. 2002;8:42–51. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Carr CE. Processing of temporal information in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1993;16:223–243. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Crist RE, Kapadia MK, Westheimer G, Gilbert CD. Perceptual learning of spatial localization: Specifiy for orientation, position, and context. J Neurophysiol. 1997;78:2889–2894. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Doupe AJ, Kuhl PK. Birdsong and human speech: Common themes and mechanisms. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1999;22:567–631. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Fahle M, Edelman S, Poggio T. Fast perceptual learning in hyperacuity. Vision Res. 1995;35:3003–3013. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Ghose GM, Yang T, Maunsell JHR. Physiological correlates of perceptual learning in monkey V1 and V2. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:1867–1888. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbon J, Malapani C, Dale CL, Gallistel CR. Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: Advances and challenges. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1997;7:170–184. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert CD, Sigman M, Crist RE. The neural basis of perceptual learning. Neuron. 2001;31:681–697. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Godfrey DA, Kiang NYS, Norris BE. Single unit activity in the posteroventral cochlear nucleus of the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1975a;162:247–268. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- ————— Single unity activity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1975b;162:269–284. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Grondin S, Meilleur-Wells G, Ouellette C, Macar F. Sensory effects on judgments of short time-intervals. Psychol Res. 1998;61:261–268. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Harrington DL, Haaland KY, Hermanowicz N. Temporal processing in the basal ganglia. Neuropsychology. 1998a;12:3–12. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Harrington DL, Haaland KY, Knight RT. Cortical networks underlying mechanisms of time perception. J Neurosci. 1998b;18:1085–1095. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Ivry R. Cerebellar involvement in the explicit representation of temporal information. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1993;682:214–230. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- ————— The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1996;6:851–857. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Ivry R, Keele SW. Timing functions of the cerebellum. J Cogn Neurosci. 1989;1:136–152. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Jeffress LA. A theory of sound localization. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1948;41:35–59. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Jueptner M, Rijntjes C, Weiller C, Faiss JH, Timmann D, Mueller SP, Diener HC. Location of a cerebellar timing process using PET. Neurology. 1995;45:1540–1545. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Karni A, Sagi D. Where practice makes perfect in texture discrimination: Evidence for primary visual cortex plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1991;88:4966–4970. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Kiang NYS, Pfeiffer RR, Warr WB, Backus ASN. Stimulus coding in the cochlear nucleus. Trans Am Otol Soc. 1965;53:35–58. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Medina JF, Garcia KS, Nores WL, Taylor NM, Mauk MD. Timing mechanisms in the cerebellum: Testing predictions of a large-scale computer simulation. J Neurosci. 2000;20:5516–5525. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Meegan DV, Aslin RN, Jacobs RA. Motor timing learned without motor training. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:860–862. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Merzenich MM, Jenkins WM, Johnston P, Schreiner C, Miller SL, Tallal P. Temporal processing deficits of language-learning impaired children ameliorated by training. Science. 1996;271:77–81. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Nagarajan SS, Blake DT, Wright BA, Byl N, Merzenich MM. Practice-related improvements in somatosensory interval discrimination are temporally specific but generalize across skin location, hemisphere, and modality. J Neurosci. 1998;18:1559–1570. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Nuding SC, Chen G-D, Sinex DG. Monaural response properties of single neurons in the chinchilla inferior colliculus. Hear Res. 1999;131:89–106. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Onoe H, Komori M, Onoe K, Takechi H, Tsukada H, Watanabe Y. Cortical networks recruited for time perception: A monkey positron emission tomography (PET) study. Neuroimage. 2001;13:37–45. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Perrett SP, Ruiz BP, Mauk MD. Cerebellar cortex lesions disrupt learning-dependent timing of conditioned eyelid responses. J Neurosci. 1993;13:1708–1718. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips DP, Irvine DRF. Responses of single neurons in physiologically defined primary auditory cortex (AI) of the cat: Frequency tuning and responses to intensity. J Neurophysiol. 1981;45:48–58. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Rammsayer TH, Lima SD. Duration discrimination of filled and empty auditory intervals: Cognitive and perceptual factors. Percept Psychophys. 1991;50:565–574. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Recanzone GH, Guard DC, Phan ML. Frequency and intensity response properties of single neurons in the auditory cortex of the behaving macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83:2315–2331. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Schoups S, Vogels R, Quian N, Orban G. Practising orientation identification improves orientation coding in V1 neurons. Nature. 2000;412:549–553. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Shannon RV, Zeng FG, Kamath V, Wygonski J, Ekelid M. Speech recognition with primarily temporal cues. Science. 1995;270:303–304. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Sutter ML, Schreiner CE. Physiology and topography of neurons with multipeaked tuning curves in cat primary auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1991;65:1207–1226. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Tallal P, Miller SL, Bedi G, Byma G, Wang X, Nagarajan SS, Schreiner C, Jenkins WM, Merzenich MM. Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science. 1996;271:81–84. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Westheimer G. Discrimination of short time intervals by the human observer. Exp Brain Res. 1999;129:121–126. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
- Wright BA, Buonomano DV, Mahncke HW, Merzenich MM. Learning and generalization of auditory temporal-interval discrimination in humans. J Neurosci. 1997;17:3956–3963. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Articles from Learning & Memory are provided here courtesy of Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press
Full text links
Read article at publisher's site: https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.55503
Read article for free, from open access legal sources, via Unpaywall: http://learnmem.cshlp.org/content/10/2/141.full.pdf
Citations & impact
Impact metrics
Citations of article over time
Smart citations by scite.ai
Explore citation contexts and check if this article has been
supported or disputed.
https://scite.ai/reports/10.1101/lm.55503
Article citations
Vagus nerve stimulation recruits the central cholinergic system to enhance perceptual learning.
Nat Neurosci, 27(11):2152-2166, 16 Sep 2024
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 39284963
Pre-supplementary Motor Cortex Mediates Learning Transfer from Perceptual to Motor Timing.
J Neurosci, 44(8):e3191202023, 21 Feb 2024
Cited by: 2 articles | PMID: 38123361 | PMCID: PMC10883661
Shift in excitation-inhibition balance underlies perceptual learning of temporal discrimination.
Neuropsychologia, 195:108814, 03 Feb 2024
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 38316210
Perceptual, procedural, and task learning for an auditory temporal discrimination task.
J Acoust Soc Am, 153(3):1823, 01 Mar 2023
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 37002097 | PMCID: PMC10257527
Adaptive Syllable Training Improves Phoneme Identification in Older Listeners with and without Hearing Loss.
Audiol Res, 12(6):653-673, 18 Nov 2022
Cited by: 2 articles | PMID: 36412658 | PMCID: PMC9680330
Go to all (74) article citations
Similar Articles
To arrive at the top five similar articles we use a word-weighted algorithm to compare words from the Title and Abstract of each citation.
Learning and generalization of auditory temporal-interval discrimination in humans.
J Neurosci, 17(10):3956-3963, 01 May 1997
Cited by: 156 articles | PMID: 9133413 | PMCID: PMC6573705
Auditory cortical plasticity in learning to discriminate modulation rate.
J Neurosci, 27(10):2663-2672, 01 Mar 2007
Cited by: 49 articles | PMID: 17344404 | PMCID: PMC4096344
Generalization to untrained conditions following training with identical stimuli.
J Basic Clin Physiol Pharmacol, 19(3-4):223-236, 01 Jan 2008
Cited by: 3 articles | PMID: 19025033
Influence of the interstimulus interval on temporal processing and learning: testing the state-dependent network model.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 364(1525):1865-1873, 01 Jul 2009
Cited by: 34 articles | PMID: 19487189 | PMCID: PMC2685819
Review Free full text in Europe PMC