Europe PMC

This website requires cookies, and the limited processing of your personal data in order to function. By using the site you are agreeing to this as outlined in our privacy notice and cookie policy.

Abstract 


Pavlovian fear conditioning provides one of the best rodent models of acquired anxiety disorders, including posttraumatic stress disorder. Injection of a variety of drugs after training in fear-conditioning paradigms can impair consolidation of fear memories. Indeed, early clinical trials suggest that immediate administration of such drugs after a traumatic event may decrease the risk of developing posttraumatic stress disorder in humans (Pitman et al., 2002; Vaiva et al., 2003). The use of such a treatment is limited by the difficulty of treating every patient at risk and by the difficulty in predicting which patients will experience chronic adverse consequences. Recent clinical trials suggest that administration of glucocorticoids may have a beneficial effect on established posttraumatic stress disorder (Aerni et al., 2004) and specific phobia (Soravia et al., 2006). Conversely, glucocorticoid administration after training is known to enhance memory consolidation (McGaugh and Roozendaal, 2002; Roozendaal, 2002). From a clinical perspective, enhancement of a fear memory or a reactivated fear memory would not be desirable. We report here that when glucocorticoids are administered immediately after reactivation of a contextual fear memory, subsequent recall is significantly diminished. Additional experiments support the interpretation that glucocorticoids not only decrease fear memory retrieval but, in addition, augment consolidation of fear memory extinction rather than decreasing reconsolidation. These findings provide a rodent model for a potential treatment of established acquired anxiety disorders in humans, as suggested by others (Aerni et al., 2004; Schelling et al., 2004), based on a mechanism of enhanced extinction.

Free full text 


Logo of jneurosciThis ArticleAbout the JournalFor AuthorsSign up for AlertsThe Journal of NeuroscienceSociety for Neuroscience
J Neurosci. 2006 Sep 13; 26(37): 9560–9566.
PMCID: PMC3917138
NIHMSID: NIHMS64517
PMID: 16971540

Postreactivation Glucocorticoids Impair Recall of Established Fear Memory

Abstract

Pavlovian fear conditioning provides one of the best rodent models of acquired anxiety disorders, including posttraumatic stress disorder. Injection of a variety of drugs after training in fear-conditioning paradigms can impair consolidation of fear memories. Indeed, early clinical trials suggest that immediate administration of such drugs after a traumatic event may decrease the risk of developing posttraumatic stress disorder in humans (Pitman et al., 2002; Vaiva et al., 2003). The use of such a treatment is limited by the difficulty of treating every patient at risk and by the difficulty in predicting which patients will experience chronic adverse consequences. Recent clinical trials suggest that administration of glucocorticoids may have a beneficial effect on established posttraumatic stress disorder (Aerni et al., 2004) and specific phobia (Soravia et al., 2006). Conversely, glucocorticoid administration after training is known to enhance memory consolidation (McGaugh and Roozendaal, 2002; Roozendaal, 2002). From a clinical perspective, enhancement of a fear memory or a reactivated fear memory would not be desirable. We report here that when glucocorticoids are administered immediately after reactivation of a contextual fear memory, subsequent recall is significantly diminished. Additional experiments support the interpretation that glucocorticoids not only decrease fear memory retrieval but, in addition, augment consolidation of fear memory extinction rather than decreasing reconsolidation. These findings provide a rodent model for a potential treatment of established acquired anxiety disorders in humans, as suggested by others (Aerni et al., 2004; Schelling et al., 2004), based on a mechanism of enhanced extinction.

Keywords: learning, memory, reconsolidation, consolidation, fear conditioning, recall, extinction, glucocorticoid

Introduction

Newly formed fear memories undergo a process of consolidation at the cellular level immediately after training (Nader et al., 2000a; Abel and Lattal, 2001; Dudai, 2004). This consolidation process is required for long-term maintenance of the memory trace. Protein synthesis inhibitors and other pharmacologic agents interfere with memory consolidation (Flexner et al., 1965; Davis and Squire, 1984; Bourtchouladze et al., 1998; McGaugh, 2000). Growing evidence suggests that fear memories have a selective sensitivity to pharmacologic interventions, including protein synthesis inhibition, after reactivation, in a manner that can negatively affect subsequent memory retrieval (Schneider and Sherman, 1968; Przybyslawski and Sara, 1997; Nader et al., 2000a; Sara, 2000; Pedreira and Maldonado, 2003; Suzuki et al., 2004). Of particular note, established memories may also be affected during reactivation through extinction, another process amenable to pharmacologic manipulation (Bouton, 1993; Myers and Davis, 2002).

Although the process of consolidation and processes occurring after reactivation require overlapping molecular pathways, a recent study suggests there are molecularly distinct processes involved in each (Lee et al., 2004). Likewise, extinction consolidation appears to be distinct in many respects compared with initial fear memory consolidation (Bouton, 1993; Myers and Davis, 2002). Pharmacologic modulation of the reactivation process to alter subsequent recall has not been fully characterized despite its potential as a feasible therapeutic target. Glucocorticoid administration in humans has been shown recently to be a relevant pharmacologic treatment of established disorders of emotional memories such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and phobias (Aerni et al., 2004; Soravia et al., 2006). These effects of glucocorticoids appear to involve both inhibition of memory retrieval and facilitation of extinction (Aerni et al., 2004; Soravia et al., 2006). Here, we demonstrate that the endogenous stress hormone, corticosterone (Cort), can reduce subsequent recall of an established contextual fear memory in mice after a single-trial reactivation, and that this effect is dependent on both reactivation of the memory and timing of corticosterone administration relative to memory reactivation. In addition, corticosterone can decrease single-trial fear memory retrieval without affecting underlying memory stability, whereas repeated retrieval in the presence of glucocorticoids can augment extinction of the contextual fear memory. These studies implicate multiple overlapping mechanisms of action for glucocorticoids in the treatment of acquired anxiety disorders.

Materials and Methods

Behavior.

Fear conditioning was performed essentially as described previously (Powell et al., 2004). Briefly, 8-week-old male C57BL/6 mice from Jackson Laboratories (Bar Harbor, ME) were placed in a Plexiglas shock box with clear front and rear walls (MedAssociates, Georgia, VT) for 2 min, and then a 30 s, 90 dB tone coterminating in a 2 s, 0.5 mA footshock was delivered twice with a 1 min interstimulus interval. Mice remained in the context for 2 min before returning to their home cage. After different intervals, as described here, freezing behavior in the training context was monitored every 10 s for 5 min by an observer blind to the experimental manipulation to measure contextual fear conditioning. Student's t test was used for two-group comparisons, whereas one-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey's tests were used in experiments with multiple groups. Significance was taken as p < 0.05. Extinction training involved daily 5 min exposures to the training context.

Because glucocorticoids blocked expression of fear memory retrieval, we examined the locomotor activity, anxiety state, and nociception effects of glucocorticoids to ensure that we were not simply affecting behavioral expression of the memory. Open-field locomotor activity was performed for 10 min in a 48 × 48 × 48 cm white plastic arena placed on the floor with fluorescent, ceiling-mounted lighting and analyzed using automated video tracking software from Noldus (Ethovision 2.3.19; Noldus Information Technology, Sterling, VA) to track velocity, distance moved, and time spent in the “center” zone, defined as a 15 × 15 cm square in the center of the arena. Elevated plus maze behavior was tested by placing mice in the center of a black Plexiglas elevated plus maze with white floors (each arm was 33 cm long and 5 cm wide with 25 cm high walls on closed arms) in a dimly lit room for 5 min as described previously (Powell et al., 2004). Each session was videotaped for later analysis by an observer blind to the genotype of the mice. Time spent in the open and closed arms, number of open and closed arm entries, time spent in the middle, and number of explorations of the open arm (defined as placing head and two limbs into open arm without full entry) was calculated. The apparatus was wiped with 70% ethanol and air-dried between mice. Accelerating rotarod behavior was performed as described previously (Powell et al., 2004). Briefly, an accelerating rotarod designed for mice (IITC Life Science, Woodland Hills, CA) was used. The rotarod was activated after placing mice on the motionless rod. The rod accelerated from 0–45 revolutions per min in 60 s. The time to fall off the rod or to turn one full revolution was measured. Five trials were performed 20–30 min apart in succession. Data were analyzed with two-way ANOVA.

Footshock sensitivity was performed by placing mice in the contextual conditioning chamber for a 2 min habituation period followed by a 2 s footshock every 30 s. The shock amplitude started at 0.05 mA and was increased by 0.05 mA with successive stimuli. The stimulus amplitudes required to elicit behavioral responses of flinching, jumping, and vocalizing were recorded.

Drug preparation/administration.

A corticosterone (Sigma, St. Louis, MO) and anisomycin (Aniso) stock solution was prepared for each dose in normal saline with 5% ethanol. Stock solutions of corticosterone and anisomycin were kept at 4°C in light-tight boxes and were made the day before the experiment or the morning of the experiment. Mice were weighed on the morning of the experiment and injected intraperitoneally with volumes ranging from 0.3 to 0.5 ml. Doses used were corticosterone 0.1, 1, 3, and 10 mg/kg based on pilot data. Three and 10 mg/kg corticosterone were used in follow-up experiments because of their effects in our dose–response experiment (see Fig. 1). Anisomycin, 150 mg/kg, was chosen as a dose at the high range of anisomycin reconsolidation effects in mice (Lattal and Abel, 2004).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is zns0370622430001.jpg

Postretrieval corticosterone transiently impairs subsequent recall of contextual fear conditioning. a, Percentage of time spent freezing in the training context before training in fear conditioning (Baseline), during subsequent reactivation 48 h hence (48 h from Train), and 24 h after reactivation (72 h from Train). Reactivation was followed by injection of vehicle or corticosterone at the indicated dose (one-way ANOVA for 72 h time point, p < 0.001, F(3,28) = 10.06; post hoc Tukey's test, p < 0.01 for Cort 3 mg/kg vs vehicle, p < 0.001, for Cort 10 mg/kg vs vehicle, p = 0.65, for Cort 3 mg/kg vs Aniso, p = 0.96, for Cort 10 mg/kg vs Aniso, p < 0.001, for Aniso vs vehicle, p = 0.90, for Cort 3 mg/kg vs Cort 10 mg/kg). b, Subsequent contextual memory recall is blocked by higher doses of corticosterone, and this effect resembles that of anisomycin. A dose–response curve for corticosterone administered immediately after memory reactivation is shown. Bars represent percentage of freezing 72 h after training (24 h after memory reactivation) (one-way ANOVA, p < 0.0001, F(6,49) = 10.86; post hoc Tukey's test, vehicle vs no injection, p = 0.95, vs Cort 0.3 mg/kg, p = 0.11, vs Cort 1 mg/kg, p = 0.99, vs Cort 3 mg/kg, p < 0.05, vs Cort 10 mg/kg, p < 0.01, vs Aniso, p < 0.001). No Injection was significantly different from Aniso, Cort 3 mg/kg, and Cort 10 mg/kg (p < 0.001 for all) and not different from Vehicle, Cort 0.3 mg/kg, or Cort 1 mg/kg (p = 0.95, 0.15, and 0.92, respectively). Cort 3 mg/kg differed from vehicle, Cort 1 mg/kg, and No Injection (p < 0.05, 0.05, and 0.01, respectively) but did not differ from Cort 0.3 mg/kg or Aniso (p = 0.39 and 0.92, respectively). Similarly, Cort 10 mg/kg differed from vehicle, Cort 1 mg/kg, and No Injection (p < 0.01, 0.01, and 0.001, respectively) but did not differ from Cort 0.3 mg/kg or Aniso (p = 0.11 and 0.99, respectively). *p < 0.05 on post hoc Tukey's test versus vehicle and No Injection. c, Effect of corticosterone on subsequent memory recall is transient. Error bars represent freezing 120 h (5 d) after training (72 h after memory reactivation). When tested 72 h after reactivation, the anisomycin effect on subsequent recall remained, whereas that of corticosterone is no longer present (ANOVA main effect of drug, p < 0.01, F(2,21) = 7.72; post hoc Tukey's test vehicle vs Aniso, *p < 0.01, vehicle vs Cort, p = 0.34; Aniso vs Cort, p = 0.058). Although b and c were necessarily performed in separate experiments in different groups on different days and thus normalized to separate control groups, a Student's t test comparison between Cort 3 mg/kg at 24 h from b and Cort 3 mg/kg at 72 h from c revealed a p = 0.068, whereas a similar comparison of Aniso 150 mg/kg at 24 h from b and Aniso 150 mg/kg from c revealed a p = 0.94. n = 8 in all groups. Error bars represent SEM in all figures.

Corticosterone measurement.

Corticosterone measurements were performed following those of de Quervain et al. (1998). C57BL/6 mice at 8 weeks of age were injected intraperitoneally with vehicle or corticosterone (Sigma) at 0.3, 1.0, 3.0, or 10 mg/kg at time 0. Thirty minutes after injection, the mice were killed, and the trunk blood was collected in heparinized tubes and stored on ice. Plasma was isolated via centrifugation at 5000 rpm for 10 min and stored on ice until freezing at −20°C before corticosterone measurement (n = 8/group). Corticosterone measurements were performed on plasma using Corticosterone Radioimmunoassay kit (B3–163; Endocrine Sciences, Tarzana, CA).

Results

We trained male C57BL/6 mice in a classical fear conditioning paradigm in which a novel environment is paired with footshock. Forty-eight hours later, re-exposure to the training environment elicited significant fear responses indicating reactivation of a learned association between this environment and the aversive footshock stimulus (Fig. 1a, 48 h). Two to 5 min after this reactivation, mice were injected with various doses of corticosterone (0.3, 1.0, 3.0, and 10.0 mg/kg), anisomycin (150 mg/kg), or vehicle and tested 24 h later for contextual fear memory. Mice injected with 3.0 and 10.0 mg/kg corticosterone after memory reactivation showed significantly decreased contextual fear memory 24 h later (p < 0.01 and 0.001, respectively) that was indistinguishable in magnitude from the effect of anisomycin (p = 0.65 and 0.96 for Cort 3 mg/kg and 10 mg/kg, respectively) (Fig. 1a). Mice injected with vehicle and mice injected with lower doses of corticosterone after memory reactivation exhibited no significant change in subsequent fear memory (p = 0.11 for Cort 0.3 mg/kg and p = 0.91 for Cort 1 mg/kg vs vehicle) (Fig. 1b). Thus, corticosteroids impair subsequent recall of a reactivated fear memory trace. The effect of glucocorticoid administration after reactivation on subsequent recall may be attributable to decreasing a reconsolidation process or augmentation of extinction.

To distinguish between a reconsolidation effect and an extinction effect, we next examined the duration of the glucocorticoid effect on subsequent recall to determine whether there was a deficit in the underlying stability of the memory trace. We tested the effects of glucocorticoid administration 72 h after the reactivation and found no significant effect on subsequent memory recall (p = 0.34) (Fig. 1c). However, the anisomycin effect on subsequent memory recall persisted (p < 0.01) (Fig. 1c). This finding suggests that glucocorticoids impair subsequent recall of a memory trace only transiently when paired with reactivation, and the duration of this effect differs from that of anisomycin. The spontaneous recovery of subsequent memory recall and the transient nature of the effect compared with anisomycin suggest an effect on extinction.

If glucocorticoids selectively impair subsequent recall of only reactivated memory traces, then administration of corticosterone 48 h after training in the absence of fear memory reactivation should not affect subsequent memory. When mice were trained in contextual fear conditioning and injected 48 h later with 3.0 mg/kg or 10.0 mg/kg corticosterone in the absence of fear memory reactivation, no effect was observed on subsequent memory measured 24 h after injection (p = 0.88 and 0.31, respectively) (Fig. 2). Thus, the effect of glucocorticoids on subsequent fear memory recall is dependent on reactivation of the memory trace and is not simply a result of prolonged effects of glucocorticoids alone.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is zns0370622430002.jpg

Reactivation of the memory is necessary for the corticosterone effect on subsequent recall. Mean percentage time spent freezing in contextual memory test 24 h after injection of corticosterone or vehicle is shown (72 h from Train). ANOVA revealed no main effect of drug (p = 0.33; F(2,33) = 1.16). Because there was no main effect, post hoc Tukey's test revealed no significant differences between vehicle versus Cort 3 mg/kg or Cort 10 mg/kg (p = 0.88 and 0.31, respectively) and no differences between Cort 3 mg/kg versus Cort 10 mg/kg (p = 0.57). n = 12 in all groups.

Extinction of a fear memory can be reversed by exposure to a “reminder shock” that is below the threshold for de novo fear conditioning, whereas reconsolidation effects theoretically cannot. To further distinguish between effects on extinction versus reconsolidation, we repeated our initial experiments giving vehicle, corticosterone, and anisomycin after reactivation. In half of the animals, however, we interposed a subthreshold (0.3 mA × 1) reminder shock 4 h after memory reactivation. The reminder shock resulted in no significant contextual fear conditioning in naive mice (data not shown). As expected for an effect on extinction, the effect of corticosterone was completely reversed by interposing a reminder shock 4 h after reactivation (p = 0.91 vs Vehicle) (Fig. 3). The anisomycin group was unaffected by the reminder shock as expected for its effect on reconsolidation (p = 0.47 for Aniso with reminder shock vs Aniso without reminder shock). These data argue for an effect of corticosterone on extinction learning rather than reconsolidation.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is zns0370622430003.jpg

a, Reminder shock rescues post-reactivation glucocorticoid effect on subsequent memory and spares that of anisomycin. ANOVA at the 72 h time point after reminder shock reveals a main effect of drug (p < 0.00001; F(2,33) = 19.52). Post hoc Tukey's test shows significant differences between vehicle and Aniso and between Cort and Aniso (p < 0.001 for both). There was no significant difference between vehicle and Cort with reminder shock (p = 0.91). ***p < 0.001 versus corresponding vehicle control. No significant differences were observed in pretrain baseline or 48 h posttrain groups (n = 12 in all groups). b, Comparison of reminder shock (+RS) versus no-reminder shock (no RS) groups on the final day of testing for each drug. ***p < 0.001 versus corresponding vehicle control. Two-way ANOVA: main effect of drug, F(2,66) = 37.25, p < 0.001; main effect of RS/no RS, F(1,66) = 13.76, p < 0.001; interaction, F(2,66) = 5.42, p < 0.01. Post hoc Tukey's test indicated significant differences between Cort/no RS, Aniso/RS, and Aniso/no RS versus all other groups and vice versa; p < 0.001 for all post hoc differences. Nonsignificant p values were 0.36 for Cort/no RS versus Aniso/no RS, 0.47 for Aniso/RS versus Aniso/no RS, 0.99 for Aniso/RS versus Cort/no RS, 0.99 for Cort/RS versus Vehicle/no RS, and 0.99 for Vehicle/RS versus Vehicle/no RS (n = 12 in all groups). No significant differences were observed in pretrain baseline or 48 h posttrain groups.

Administration of corticosterone 30 min prior to contextual fear memory reactivation impaired retrieval of the fear memory during reactivation (p < 0.01) (Fig. 4a). Measurement of corticosterone levels 30 min after intraperitoneal injection indicate that a 3.0 mg/kg injection produced corticosterone levels well within the range observed with physiologic stressors in mice (supplemental Table 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material) (Pawlak et al., 2003; Hebda-Bauer et al., 2004).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is zns0370622430004.jpg

Corticosterone impairs acute retrieval of contextual fear memory without affecting the stability of the underlying memory trace. a, Injection of corticosterone 30 min before memory test impairs retrieval. Results of “Probe a” (contextual memory test performed 48 h after training; percentage freezing in context 30 min after corticosterone injection) are shown. ANOVA revealed a main effect of drug (p < 0.01, F(2,33) = 7.49). Post hoc Tukey's test revealed significant differences between vehicle versus Cort 3 mg/kg (p < 0.01) and versus Cort 10 mg/kg (p < 0.01). No significant difference between Cort 3 mg/kg and Cort 10 mg/kg was observed (p = 0.99). **p < 0.01; n = 12 in all groups. b, Impairment of memory retrieval by corticosterone had no significant effect on subsequent memory. Results of “Probe b” (contextual memory test performed 72 h after training or 24 h after Probe a; percentage freezing in context 24 h) are shown. ANOVA revealed no main effect of drug (p = 0.28; F(2,33) = 1.33). Because there was no main effect, post hoc Tukey's test revealed no significant differences between vehicle versus Cort 3 mg/kg or Cort 10 mg/kg (p = 0.36 and 0.33, respectively) and no differences between Cort 3 mg/kg versus Cort 10 mg/kg (p = 0.99). When freezing in the Cort groups at 48 h in a (retrieval block by Cort) was compared with that of Cort groups at 72 h in b (memory after retrieval block by Cort), there was no significant difference between Cort groups with retrieval blocked (a) and Cort groups after retrieval blockade (b) (p = 0.26 and 0.23 for 3 mg/kg and 10 mg/kg, respectively).

Administration of glucocorticoids prior to memory reactivation, which impairs acute retrieval (Fig. 4a), does not significantly affect subsequent recall in the absence of glucocorticoids (Fig. 4b). Mice were trained in the fear conditioning paradigm on day 1 and administered 3.0 mg/kg or 10 mg/kg corticosterone 30 min before memory reactivation 48 h later, which led to a significantly impaired recall response (p < 0.01) (Fig. 4a). On repeat fear memory testing in the absence of drug 24 h after blocking retrieval with corticosterone (72 h after training), context-dependent fear conditioning was not statistically different from controls (p = 0.36 for 3 mg/kg and 0.33 for 10 mg/kg) (Fig. 4b), although a trend toward a decrease was noted. Because Figure 4a indicated a block of retrieval and Figure 4b indicated no subsequent difference between controls and Cort groups, we conclude that corticosterone blocks retrieval without affecting subsequent memory expression. Although there was clearly a difference between retrieval in the presence of Cort (Fig. 4a) and no difference in subsequent retrieval (Fig. 4b), there was not a significant difference in this experiment between freezing levels in the Cort groups in Figure 4, a and b. Because of this discrepancy, we repeated this experiment with 10 mg/kg corticosterone and found the same result of a significant block of retrieval and no significant impairment in subsequent memory (Student's t test, p < 0.05 for retrieval and p = 0.63 for subsequent memory expression). In this additional experiment, there was a significant difference between 10 mg/kg Cort during retrieval (48 h after training) and the postretrieval memory expression (72 h after training; p = 0.01; n = 6) (data not shown). Thus, under our particular experimental conditions, blocking a single contextual memory retrieval does not have a significant effect on subsequent expression of the memory. It seems that glucocorticoids must be administered after successful reactivation of the fear memory to impair subsequent recall after a single reactivation trial, although a trend toward reduced subsequent memory was observed. It is possible, however, that with a different experimental protocol, one might find a significant one-trial effect of pre-reactivation corticosterone on subsequent memory.

The effects of glucocorticoids on subsequent fear memory recall and on acute retrieval are not caused by nonspecific alterations in behavior but are rather a selective effect of glucocorticoids on these fear memory processes themselves. Acute administration of 3.0 mg/kg corticosterone, a dose that can impair fear memory retrieval or subsequent memory, depending on the timing of application, does not alter open-field locomotor activity or open field measures of anxiety such as time spent in the center (supplemental Fig. 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). The same dose of corticosterone also has no effect on anxiety measures in the elevated plus maze (supplemental Fig. 2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), gross motor coordination in the accelerating rotarod (supplemental Fig. 3, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), or footshock sensitivity (supplemental Fig. 4, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). These negative findings in mice are interesting in light of previous findings that glucocorticoids increase anxiety/arousal in human subjects (Abercrombie et al., 2005). It may be that our tests of anxiety in rodents are not as sensitive as measures in human studies. These control data further support a selective effect of glucocorticoids on fear memory retrieval processes.

Although the effects of glucocorticoids given before a single reactivation trial do not significantly affect subsequent memory recall, we did observe a trend toward a decrease in subsequent memory (Fig. 4b). Based on this result, we hypothesized that reactivation of fear memory during glucocorticoid inhibition of retrieval might enhance the extinction process after multiple reactivations. Thus, we tested the effects of repeated corticosterone block of retrieval on extinction. Three days of repeated corticosterone administration before reactivation (context exposure) trials led to decreased fear memory expression (p < 0.05) (Fig. 5a). When the underlying memory trace was examined 1 d later in the absence of corticosterone, inhibition of fear expression remained enhanced (p < 0.01) (Fig. 5a). Thus, glucocorticoids may affect subsequent recall of an established memory if given after a single reactivation and may also augment extinction if given before multiple reactivation trials. In this case, the inhibition of fear expression could be a result of a direct effect of corticosterone to enhance consolidation of extinction or resulting from blockade of fear memory retrieval during extinction. Repeated trials of post-reactivation corticosterone led to a similar effect (p < 0.01) (Fig. 5b), indicating a likely effect on consolidation of extinction memory, although a separate effect of decreasing retrieval on extinction cannot be excluded. Interestingly, multiple trials of post-reactivation corticosterone prolonged the extinction effect. Although single-trial post-reactivation corticosterone effects showed spontaneous recovery of memory by 72 h (Fig. 1c), multiple-trial post-reactivation corticosterone effects lasted at least 72 h (p < 0.001) (Fig. 5b). Both pre-retrieval and post-retrieval corticosterone effects with multiple trials were dependent on reactivation of the memory and were not simply an effect of repeated corticosterone administration (p = 0.97) (Fig. 5c).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is zns0370622430005.jpg

Corticosterone augments multiple-trial extinction. a, Corticosterone blockade of retrieval augments extinction. This effect is dependent on multiple reactivation trials (compare with Fig. 4b). Repeated injection of corticosterone 30 min before extinction trials for 3 d impairs both retrieval acutely and subsequent memory recall tested 24 h after the last extinction trial in the absence of corticosterone. ANOVA with repeated measures on days 1–3 indicates a main effect of drug (F(1,18) = 6.29; p < 0.05). Student's t test of day 4 indicates p < 0.01. Individual t tests for days 1, 2, and 3 indicate p = 0.14, p < 0.05, and p < 0.05, respectively. n = 10 in all groups. b, Multiple trials of post-reactivation corticosterone further augment extinction measured 24 h after the third trial (Trial 4) and prolongs the extinction effect (Probe, measured 72 h after Trial 4). Although a single trial of post-reactivation corticosterone has an effect on subsequent memory, multiple trials seem to enhance this effect (Trial 4). A memory test 3 d hence reveals a longer-lasting effect of post-reactivation corticosterone after multiple reactivation trials (Probe) compared with single-trial reactivation (Fig. 1). Repeated-measures ANOVA reveals main effects of drug (F(1,22) = 13.88; p < 0.01), day (F(3,66) = 42.09, p < 0.001), and interaction between drug and day (F(3,66) = 14.69; p < 0.001). Student's t test of days 1–4 and 7 indicate p = 0.75, p < 0.01, p < 0.001, p < 0.001, and p < 0.001, respectively. c, The effect of corticosterone on extinction with multiple trials requires memory reactivation. Multiple daily injections of corticosterone in the absence of memory reactivation do not affect subsequent memory recall. Student's t test of probe: p = 0.97; n = 12 in all groups.

Discussion

These findings demonstrate that administration of the endogenous stress hormone, corticosterone, after a single memory reactivation can impair subsequent recall of an established contextual fear memory. Previous studies have implicated glucocorticoids in modulation of extinction of other types of fear memory (Bohus and Lissak, 1968; Micheau et al., 1982; Barrett and Gonzalez-Lima, 2004; Moreira et al., 2004; Yang et al., 2006), primarily using cue-elicited fear memory. The facilitation of contextual fear memory extinction may have particular relevance for acquired anxiety disorders. Interestingly, the effect of glucocorticoids on subsequent recall is the opposite of its effect on memory consolidation. Such a post-reactivation effect of a drug may be interpreted as either inhibition of reconsolidation or as augmentation of extinction learning.

Post-reactivation corticosterone may act by inhibiting reconsolidation of the reactivated memory. Memory consolidation and reactivation-induced memory “lability” appear to be dependent on some overlapping molecular mechanisms such as NMDA receptor activation, β-adrenergic receptor activation, and cAMP response element-binding protein activation, whereas some manipulations can affect one process and not the other (Bourtchuladze et al., 1994; Przybyslawski and Sara, 1997; Przybyslawski et al., 1999; Cahill et al., 2000; Sara, 2000; Kida et al., 2002; McGaugh, 2002; McGaugh and Roozendaal, 2002; Debiec and Ledoux, 2004; Lee et al., 2004). Certainly, the initial effect of corticosterone is similar in magnitude to the effect of anisomycin, a drug which empirically defines reconsolidation effects in rodents (Nader et al., 2000a,b; Sara, 2000; Debiec et al., 2002). Also, the effect of corticosterone, like that of anisomycin, is only present after reactivation of the memory and not in the absence of reactivation. Indeed, blocking retrieval of the memory seems to decrease the effect of corticosterone on subsequent memory. However, the effect of single-trial post-reactivation corticosterone is transient. In theory, a drug that decreases the reconsolidation of a memory is predicted to have a lasting effect. In many reports, the effect of anisomycin on reconsolidation is sustained (Lattal and Abel, 2001; Debiec et al., 2002), although other studies demonstrate spontaneous recovery of memory after various post-retrieval manipulations, including anisomycin (Miller and Springer, 1974; Mactutus et al., 1979; Judge and Quartermain, 1982; Anokhin et al., 2002; Lattal and Abel, 2004). Although some have suggested that preservation of the underlying memory after postretrieval manipulations might be incorporated into theories of reconsolidation (Lattal and Abel, 2004), corticosterone does not permanently impair recall performance.

Another interpretation is that corticosterone is augmenting consolidation of contextual fear memory extinction, an effect observed in studies of other forms of memory (Bohus and Lissak, 1968; Micheau et al., 1982; Barrett and Gonzalez-Lima, 2004; Moreira et al., 2004; Yang et al., 2006). One-trial extinction of contextual fear conditioning would be exceptional and does not occur under our control conditions (Fig. 1a, “vehicle”). It is possible, however, that corticosterone is acting to augment consolidation of extinction memory such that one trial is sufficient to induce weak extinction that is only transiently expressed. One property of extinction is that the original memory can show spontaneous recovery (Bouton, 1993; Myers and Davis, 2002), similar to the transient effect of post-reactivation corticosterone. The need for reactivation of memory for the effect is also consistent with a need for reactivation of memory in the extinction process. Finally, the extinction process can be overcome by a subthreshold reminder shock, whereas a deficit in reconsolidation cannot. The post-reactivation glucocorticoid effect on contextual fear memory is reversed by reminder shock in our hands, unlike that of anisomycin. Thus, augmentation of single-trial contextual fear memory extinction is the more likely mechanism for post-reactivation effects of corticosterone on subsequent memory.

Augmentation of contextual fear extinction by glucocorticoids is consistent with several previous findings in the literature. For example, corticosterone is known to increase consolidation of other forms of memory (McGaugh and Roozendaal, 2002; Roozendaal, 2002). Similarly, previous reports suggest that pre-reactivation and post-reactivation glucocorticoids may play a role in the treatment of PTSD and phobia (Aerni et al., 2004; Soravia et al., 2006). Furthermore, extinction may be modulated by glucocorticoids in other forms of memory (Bohus and Lissak, 1968; Micheau et al., 1982; Barrett and Gonzalez-Lima, 2004; Moreira et al., 2004; Yang et al., 2006)

Similar to previous findings (de Quervain et al., 1998; Roozendaal et al., 2004), we demonstrated that glucocorticoids block fear memory retrieval in a contextual fear-conditioning paradigm. We extended this finding by demonstrating that glucocorticoids given before a single-trial retrieval do not significantly affect the long-term stability of the underlying memory trace, nor do they affect control behaviors that might interfere with measures of memory retrieval. Furthermore, we confirmed observations by de Quervain et al. (1998) indicating that increased glucocorticoids in the absence of reactivation do not affect subsequent recall when glucocorticoid levels had returned to baseline (de Quervain et al., 1998). Thus, successful retrieval of fear memory is necessary for glucocorticoid effects on subsequent memory recall after a single-trial reactivation. Similar effects of glucocorticoids on memory retrieval have been observed in humans (de Quervain et al., 2000). Additionally, we have significantly extended previous findings that pre-reactivation or post-reactivation glucocorticoids can decrease repeated-trial, cued fear memories (Yang et al., 2006) by examining the time course, spontaneous recovery, comparison to anisomycin, and sensitivity to reminder shock to distinguish extinction and retrieval from reconsolidation effects on contextual fear memories.

The glucocorticoid effects are present whether the glucocorticoid is administered before or after each extinction trial (Fig. 5). Administration of glucocorticoids after a single reactivation trial likely augments consolidation of extinction. Administration of glucocorticoids 30 min before multiple reactivation trials can also enhance extinction, either by augmenting extinction consolidation or as a result of decreasing contextual fear memory retrieval.

It is tempting to speculate about the role of glucocorticoids in susceptibility to and maintenance of learned anxiety disorders such as posttraumatic stress disorder. Patients with PTSD have been shown to have lower circulating levels of corticosterone and altered hypothalamic-pituitary axis activity (Yehuda, 2002). Blunted corticosterone responses might actually lead to persistent, pathologically strong, traumatic memories in affected individuals. Repeated reactivation of memories in unaffected individuals, followed by normal corticosterone surges, may serve to weaken their ability to recall traumatic memories over time. In contrast, a blunted corticosterone response may result in blunted extinction processes with pathologically persistent fear responses to contextual cues. The longer-lasting effect of multiple-trial post-reactivation corticosterone is consistent with this possibility.

The clinical implications of pharmacologic interventions that decrease the strength of an established fear memory trace are clear. Glucocorticoids have been used in clinical trials to decrease symptoms of PTSD and phobia (Aerni et al., 2004; Soravia et al., 2006). Our data confirm that not only is the particular pharmacologic agent of importance, but that the reactivation of relevant memories, timing of administration relative to reactivation, and number of reactivation trials can have an impact on the desired outcome. The use of mice in the present study will allow directed investigation of the underlying receptor subtypes and mechanisms of the effect of glucocorticoid on established fear memories, affording the potential for more specific therapy in future trials that might allow us to avoid some side effects of a general treatment with glucocorticoids. In deference to ethical issues surrounding therapies targeted to emotional memories, it will be important in clinical trials to distinguish between suppression of the pathologically intense emotional valence of a traumatic memory versus the elimination of the declarative memory of said event.

In conclusion, glucocorticoids transiently decrease subsequent recall of an established fear memory after its reactivation, likely through augmentation of extinction in a single trial. Glucocorticoids have two effects on reactivated emotional memories. When administered before a single reactivation, memory retrieval is inhibited, whereas the stability of the underlying memory is unaffected. When drug administration follows memory reactivation, subsequent recall is dramatically reduced. Repeated administration may obviate the need for such precise timing. These studies provide a model for a therapeutic approach in the treatment of pathological emotional memories (Aerni et al., 2004; Schelling et al., 2004) and suggest future experiments designed to explore the specific molecular mechanisms of this effect.

Footnotes

This work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH065975, Conte Center Grant MH6045, the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression, and the Department of Veterans Affairs. R.W.G., C.M.P., W.-H.C., and J.B. conceived and designed the experiments; W.-H.C., J.H., and J.B. performed the experiments with input from C.M.P.; W.-H.C., J.H., J.B., and C.M.P. performed statistical analysis; and C.M.P. wrote this paper with input from W.-H.C., J.B., and R.W.G. We thank Dr. Eric J. Nestler for helpful comments.

References

Abel and Lattal, 2001. Abel T, Lattal KM. Molecular mechanisms of memory acquisition, consolidation and retrieval. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2001;11:180–187. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Abercrombie et al., 2005. Abercrombie HC, Kalin NH, Davidson RJ. Acute cortisol elevations cause heightened arousal ratings of objectively nonarousing stimuli. Emotion. 2005;5:354–359. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Aerni et al., 2004. Aerni A, Traber R, Hock C, Roozendaal B, Schelling G, Papassotiropoulos A, Nitsch RM, Schnyder U, de Quervain DJ. Low-dose cortisol for symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1488–1490. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Anokhin et al., 2002. Anokhin KV, Tiunova AA, Rose SP. Reminder effects–reconsolidation or retrieval deficit? Pharmacological dissection with protein synthesis inhibitors following reminder for a passive-avoidance task in young chicks. Eur J Neurosci. 2002;15:1759–1765. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Barrett and Gonzalez-Lima, 2004. Barrett D, Gonzalez-Lima F. Behavioral effects of metyrapone on pavlovian extinction. Neurosci Lett. 2004;371:91–96. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Bohus and Lissak, 1968. Bohus B, Lissak K. Adrenocortical hormones and avoidance behaviour of rats. Int J Neuropharmacol. 1968;7:301–306. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Bourtchuladze et al., 1994. Bourtchuladze R, Frenguelli B, Blendy J, Cioffi D, Schutz G, Silva AJ. Deficient long-term memory in mice with a targeted mutation of the cAMP-responsive element-binding protein. Cell. 1994;79:59–68. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Bourtchouladze et al., 1998. Bourtchouladze R, Abel T, Berman N, Gordon R, Lapidus K, Kandel ER. Different training procedures recruit either one or two critical periods for contextual memory consolidation, each of which requires protein synthesis and PKA. Learn Mem. 1998;5:365–374. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Bouton, 1993. Bouton ME. Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of pavlovian learning. Psychol Bull. 1993;114:80–99. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Cahill et al., 2000. Cahill L, Pham CA, Setlow B. Impaired memory consolidation in rats produced with beta-adrenergic blockade. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2000;74:259–266. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Davis and Squire, 1984. Davis HP, Squire LR. Protein synthesis and memory: a review. Psychol Bull. 1984;96:518–559. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
de Quervain et al., 1998. de Quervain DJ, Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL. Stress and glucocorticoids impair retrieval of long-term spatial memory. Nature. 1998;394:787–790. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
de Quervain et al., 2000. de Quervain DJ, Roozendaal B, Nitsch RM, McGaugh JL, Hock C. Acute cortisone administration impairs retrieval of long-term declarative memory in humans. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:313–314. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Debiec and Ledoux, 2004. Debiec J, Ledoux JE. Disruption of reconsolidation but not consolidation of auditory fear conditioning by noradrenergic blockade in the amygdala. Neuroscience. 2004;129:267–272. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Debiec et al., 2002. Debiec J, LeDoux JE, Nader K. Cellular and systems reconsolidation in the hippocampus. Neuron. 2002;36:527–538. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Dudai, 2004. Dudai Y. The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annu Rev Psychol. 2004;55:51–86. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Flexner et al., 1965. Flexner LB, Flexner JB, Stellar E. Memory and cerebral protein synthesis in mice as affected by graded amounts of puromycin. Exp Neurol. 1965;13:264–272. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Hebda-Bauer et al., 2004. Hebda-Bauer EK, Watson SJ, Akil H. CREB deficient mice show inhibition and low activity in novel environments without changes in stress reactivity. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;20:503–513. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Judge and Quartermain, 1982. Judge ME, Quartermain D. Characteristics of retrograde amnesia following reactivation of memory in mice. Physiol Behav. 1982;28:585–590. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Kida et al., 2002. Kida S, Josselyn SA, de Ortiz SP, Kogan JH, Chevere I, Masushige S, Silva AJ. CREB required for the stability of new and reactivated fear memories. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:348–355. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Lattal and Abel, 2001. Lattal KM, Abel T. Different requirements for protein synthesis in acquisition and extinction of spatial preferences and context-evoked fear. J Neurosci. 2001;21:5773–5780. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Lattal and Abel, 2004. Lattal KM, Abel T. Behavioral impairments caused by injections of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin after contextual retrieval reverse with time. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:4667–4672. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Lee et al., 2004. Lee JL, Everitt BJ, Thomas KL. Independent cellular processes for hippocampal memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Science. 2004;304:839–843. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Mactutus et al., 1979. Mactutus CF, Riccio DC, Ferek JM. Retrograde amnesia for old (reactivated) memory: some anomalous characteristics. Science. 1979;204:1319–1320. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
McGaugh, 2000. McGaugh JL. Memory–a century of consolidation. Science. 2000;287:248–251. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
McGaugh, 2002. McGaugh JL. Memory consolidation and the amygdala: a systems perspective. Trends Neurosci. 2002;25:456. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
McGaugh and Roozendaal, 2002. McGaugh JL, Roozendaal B. Role of adrenal stress hormones in forming lasting memories in the brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002;12:205–210. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Micheau et al., 1982. Micheau J, Destrade C, Soumireu-Mourat B. Posttrial injections of corticosterone in dorsal hippocampus of the BALB/c mouse facilitate extinction of appetitive operant conditioning in the Skinner box. C R Seances Acad Sci III. 1982;294:1109–1112. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Miller and Springer, 1974. Miller RR, Springer AD. Implications of recovery from experimental amnesia. Psychol Rev. 1974;81:470–473. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Moreira et al., 2004. Moreira PS, Pulman KG, Pottinger TG. Extinction of a conditioned response in rainbow trout selected for high or low responsiveness to stress. Horm Behav. 2004;46:450–457. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Myers and Davis, 2002. Myers KM, Davis M. Behavioral and neural analysis of extinction. Neuron. 2002;36:567–584. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Nader et al., 2000a. Nader K, Schafe GE, Le Doux JE. Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature. 2000a;406:722–726. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Nader et al., 2000b. Nader K, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. The labile nature of consolidation theory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2000b;1:216–219. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Pawlak et al., 2003. Pawlak R, Magarinos AM, Melchor J, McEwen B, Strickland S. Tissue plasminogen activator in the amygdala is critical for stress-induced anxiety-like behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:168–174. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Pedreira and Maldonado, 2003. Pedreira ME, Maldonado H. Protein synthesis subserves reconsolidation or extinction depending on reminder duration. Neuron. 2003;38:863–869. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Pitman et al., 2002. Pitman RK, Sanders KM, Zusman RM, Healy AR, Cheema F, Lasko NB, Cahill L, Orr SP. Pilot study of secondary prevention of posttraumatic stress disorder with propranolol. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:189–192. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Powell et al., 2004. Powell CM, Schoch S, Monteggia L, Barrot M, Matos MF, Feldmann N, Sudhof TC, Nestler EJ. The presynaptic active zone protein RIM1alpha is critical for normal learning and memory. Neuron. 2004;42:143–153. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Przybyslawski and Sara, 1997. Przybyslawski J, Sara SJ. Reconsolidation of memory after its reactivation. Behav Brain Res. 1997;84:241–246. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Przybyslawski et al., 1999. Przybyslawski J, Roullet P, Sara SJ. Attenuation of emotional and nonemotional memories after their reactivation: role of β adrenergic receptors. J Neurosci. 1999;19:6623–6628. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Roozendaal, 2002. Roozendaal B. Stress and memory: opposing effects of glucocorticoids on memory consolidation and memory retrieval. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2002;78:578–595. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Roozendaal et al., 2004. Roozendaal B, de Quervain DJ, Schelling G, McGaugh JL. A systemically administered beta-adrenoceptor antagonist blocks corticosterone-induced impairment of contextual memory retrieval in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2004;81:150–154. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Sara, 2000. Sara SJ. Retrieval and reconsolidation: toward a neurobiology of remembering. Learn Mem. 2000;7:73–84. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Schelling et al., 2004. Schelling G, Roozendaal B, De Quervain DJ. Can posttraumatic stress disorder be prevented with glucocorticoids? Ann NY Acad Sci. 2004;1032:158–166. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Schneider and Sherman, 1968. Schneider AM, Sherman W. Amnesia: a function of the temporal relation of footshock to electroconvulsive shock. Science. 1968;159:219–221. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Soravia et al., 2006. Soravia LM, Heinrichs M, Aerni A, Maroni C, Schelling G, Ehlert U, Roozendaal B, de Quervain DJ. Glucocorticoids reduce phobic fear in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:5585–5590. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Suzuki et al., 2004. Suzuki A, Josselyn SA, Frankland PW, Masushige S, Silva AJ, Kida S. Memory reconsolidation and extinction have distinct temporal and biochemical signatures. J Neurosci. 2004;24:4787–4795. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Vaiva et al., 2003. Vaiva G, Ducrocq F, Jezequel K, Averland B, Lestavel P, Brunet A, Marmar CR. Immediate treatment with propranolol decreases posttraumatic stress disorder two months after trauma. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:947–949. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Yang et al., 2006. Yang YL, Chao PK, Lu KT. Systemic and intra-amygdala administration of glucocorticoid agonist and antagonist modulate extinction of conditioned fear. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:912–924. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
Yehuda, 2002. Yehuda R. Current status of cortisol findings in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2002;25:341–368. vii. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

Citations & impact 


Impact metrics

Jump to Citations

Citations of article over time

Article citations


Go to all (136) article citations

Data 


Data behind the article

This data has been text mined from the article, or deposited into data resources.

Funding 


Funders who supported this work.

NIMH NIH HHS (6)