Europe PMC

This website requires cookies, and the limited processing of your personal data in order to function. By using the site you are agreeing to this as outlined in our privacy notice and cookie policy.

Abstract 


Importance

Schizophrenia is a highly heritable, polygenic condition characterized by a relatively diverse phenotype and frequent comorbid conditions, such as anxiety and depression. At present, limited evidence explains how genetic risk for schizophrenia is manifest in the general population.

Objective

To investigate the extent to which genetic risk for schizophrenia is associated with different phenotypes during adolescence in a population-based birth cohort.

Design, setting, and participants

This cohort study used data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Of 14,062 children in the birth cohort, genetic data were available for 9912 adolescents. Data were collected periodically from September 6, 1990, and collection is ongoing. Data were analyzed from March 4 to August 13, 2015.

Exposures

Polygenic risk scores (PRSs) for schizophrenia generated for individuals in the ALSPAC cohort using results of the second Psychiatric Genomics Consortium Schizophrenia genome-wide association study as a training set.

Main outcomes and measures

Logistic regression was used to assess associations between the schizophrenia PRS and (1) psychotic experiences (Psychosis-Like Symptom Interview at 12 and 18 years of age), (2) negative symptoms (Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences at 16.5 years of age), (3) depressive disorder (Development and Well-Being Assessment at 15.5 years of age), and (4) anxiety disorder (Development and Well-Being Assessment at 15.5 years of age) in adolescence.

Results

Of the 8230 ALSPAC participants whose genetic data passed quality control checks (51.2% male, 48.8% female), 3676 to 5444 participated in assessments from 12 to 18 years of age. The PRSs created using single-nucleotide polymorphisms with a training-set P ≤ .05 threshold were associated with negative symptoms (odds ratio [OR] per SD increase in PRS, 1.21; 95% CI, 1.08-1.36; R(2) = 0.007) and anxiety disorder (OR per SD increase in PRS, 1.17; 95% CI, 1.06- 1.29; R(2) = 0.005). No evidence was found of an association between schizophrenia PRS and psychotic experiences (OR per SD increase in PRS, 1.08; 95% CI, 0.98-1.19; R(2) = 0.001) or depressive disorder (OR per SD increase in PRS, 1.02; 95% CI, 0.91-1.13; R(2) = 0.00005). Results were mostly consistent across different training-set P value thresholds and using different cutoffs and measures of the psychopathological outcomes.

Conclusions and relevance

This study demonstrates polygenic overlaps between common genetic polymorphisms associated with schizophrenia and negative symptoms and anxiety disorder but not with psychotic experiences or depression. Because the genetic risk for schizophrenia appears to be manifest as anxiety and negative symptoms during adolescence, a greater focus on these phenotypes rather than on psychotic experiences might be required for prediction of transition in at-risk samples.

Free full text 


Logo of wtpaEurope PMCEurope PMC Funders GroupSubmit a Manuscript
JAMA Psychiatry. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC5024747
EMSID: EMS69048
PMID: 26818099

Phenotypic Manifestation of Genetic Risk for Schizophrenia During Adolescence in the General Population

Hannah J. Jones, PhD, Evie Stergiakouli, PhD, Katherine E. Tansey, PhD, Leon Hubbard, PhD, Jon Heron, PhD, Mary Cannon, MD, PhD, Peter Holmans, PhD, Glyn Lewis, PhD, David E. J. Linden, DPhil, Peter B. Jones, PhD, George Davey Smith, MD, DSc, Michael C. O'Donovan, PhD, FRCPsych, Michael J. Owen, PhD, FRCPsych, James T. Walters, MRCPsych, PhD, and Stanley Zammit, MRCPsych, PhD

Abstract

Importance

Schizophrenia is a highly heritable, polygenic condition characterized by a relatively diverse phenotype and frequent comorbid conditions, such as anxiety and depression. At present, limited evidence explains how genetic risk for schizophrenia is manifest in the general population.

Objective

To investigate the extent to which genetic risk for schizophrenia is associated with different phenotypes during adolescence in a population-based birth cohort.

Design, Setting, and Participants

This cohort study used data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Of 14 062 children in the birth cohort, genetic data were available for 9912 adolescents. Data were collected periodically from September 6, 1990, and collection is ongoing. Data were analyzed from March 4 to August 13, 2015.

Exposures

Polygenic risk scores (PRSs) for schizophrenia generated for individuals in the ALSPAC cohort using results of the second Psychiatric Genomics Consortium Schizophrenia genome-wide association study as a training set.

Main Outcomes and Measures

Logistic regression was used to assess associations between the schizophrenia PRS and (1) psychotic experiences (Psychosis-Like Symptom Interview at 12 and 18 years of age), (2) negative symptoms (Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences at 16.5 years of age), (3) depressive disorder (Development and Well-Being Assessment at 15.5 years of age), and (4) anxiety disorder (Development and Well-Being Assessment at 15.5 years of age) in adolescence.

Results

Of the 8230 ALSPAC participants whose genetic data passed quality control checks (51.2% male, 48.8% female), 3676 to 5444 participated in assessments from 12 to 18 years of age. The PRSs created using single-nucleotide polymorphisms with a training-set P ≤ .05 threshold were associated with negative symptoms (odds ratio [OR] per SD increase in PRS, 1.21; 95% CI, 1.08-1.36; R2 = 0.007) and anxiety disorder (OR per SD increase in PRS, 1.17; 95% CI, 1.06-1.29; R2 = 0.005). No evidence was found of an association between schizophrenia PRS and psychotic experiences (OR per SD increase in PRS, 1.08; 95% CI, 0.98-1.19; R2 = 0.001) or depressive disorder (OR per SD increase in PRS, 1.02; 95% CI, 0.91-1.13; R2 = 0.00005). Results were mostly consistent across different training-set P value thresholds and using different cutoffs and measures of the psychopathological outcomes.

Conclusions and Relevance

This study demonstrates polygenic overlaps between common genetic polymorphisms associated with schizophrenia and negative symptoms and anxiety disorder but not with psychotic experiences or depression. Because the genetic risk for schizophrenia appears to be manifest as anxiety and negative symptoms during adolescence, a greater focus on these phenotypes rather than on psychotic experiences might be required for prediction of transition in at-risk samples.

Schizophrenia has a heritability of approximately 80%, and genome-wide association studies (GWASs) indicate that multiple independent loci contribute to its etiology.1 The importance of studying the phenotypic manifestations of increased genetic liability for schizophrenia has been long recognized and originally involved small samples of individuals at high risk as indexed by having a family history.2 Genetic advances now provide the opportunity to extend the power and generalizability of high-risk studies into the general population by examining individuals according to genetic risk. Although individual loci have small effects on risk, multiple-loci approaches show that, cumulatively, alleles on current GWAS platforms explain one-half to one-third of the genetic risk for schizophrenia.3,4 Information from even moderately associated alleles can be collapsed into a single polygenic risk score (PRS) that can be used to explore shared genetic effects with other disorders and to examine how genetic risk is manifest early during development.5

Schizophrenia is defined by psychotic experiences (hallucinations, delusions, and thought disorder) and negative symptoms (eg, blunted affect and apathy), although cognitive deficits are also common as is comorbidity with other diagnoses, particularly affective and anxiety disorders.6 Longitudinal studies show that anxiety, depression, and cognitive deficits often predate schizophrenia,711 indicating that these phenotypes might represent early expression of genetic risk for schizophrenia. Although genetic overlap across psychiatric disorders is common,1215 knowledge of how genetic risk is expressed at different stages of the life course could foster understanding of the etiologic mechanisms and risk prediction and inform targeted interventions.

Three previous studies1618 have used a schizophrenia PRS generated from a GWAS training set that captured approximately 3% of the proportion of risk variance4 to examine associations with symptoms that characterize schizophrenia. In the first study,16 the schizophrenia PRS was not associated with symptom dimensions characteristic of the disorder in cases with schizophrenia or control individuals (while associations within the whole sample were attributed to confounding by case-control status). Similarly, another study17 reported no association with positive symptom, cognitive, and negative symptom dimensions in a general population sample of adolescents. Finally, the third study18 found no strong evidence that the schizophrenia PRS was associated with psychotic experiences at 12 years of age within the population-based Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) birth cohort. Subsequently, a larger GWAS1 has been completed that explains a much greater proportion of risk variance, thus providing greater power to examine how genetic risk is manifest during development.19 We aimed to examine the psychopathological features associated with the early expression of genetic risk for schizophrenia and, more specifically, whether a schizophrenia PRS derived from the most recent GWAS is associated with (1) psychotic experiences, (2) negative symptoms, (3) anxiety disorders, or (4) depression during adolescence in a large population-based sample.

Methods

Participants

The sample consisted of young individuals within the ALSPAC cohort. The initial cohort consisted of 14 062 children born to women residing in the former Avon Health Authority area with an expected delivery date from April 1, 1991, to December 31, 1992 (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/; all available data are given at http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/access/).20,21 Data were collected periodically from September 6, 1990, and collection is ongoing. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics Committee and the local research ethics committees (listed at http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/research-ethics/). All participants provided written informed consent.

Genetic Data

Genetic data were acquired from 9912 participants using a genome-wide single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) genotyping platform (HumanHap550-Quad; Illumina). After quality control assessment, imputation, and restriction to 1 adolescent per family, genetic data were available for 8230 individuals (more detail is found in the eMethods in the Supplement).

Measures

Psychotic Experiences

The semistructured Psychosis-Like Symptom Interview22,23 was used to assess psychotic experiences (hallucinations, delusions, or experiences of thought interference) at ages 12 and 18 years. To maximize the numbers within our sample, individuals were deemed to have a psychotic experience if rated as having 1 or more definite psychotic experiences at 12 or 18 years of age compared with no experiences or suspected psychotic experiences only at 12 or 18 years of age. More details on the Psychosis-Like Symptom Interview and on the other outcome measures used in this study are given in the eMethods in the Supplement.

Negative Symptoms

Negative symptoms were assessed using 10 questions based on items from the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences self-report questionnaire24 at 16.5 years of age (eMethods and eTable 1 in the Supplement). The questions measure negative symptoms, such as apathy, anergia, and asociality. Each item was rated on a 4-point scale (0 indicates never; 1, sometimes; 2, often; and 3, always). A total score was constructed based on the sum of responses (minimum score, 0; maximum score, 30). A binary variable was created using a total score of 14 as a cutoff that was chosen to define approximately the top decile (9.18%) of the sample.

Depressive and Anxiety Disorders

Depressive and anxiety disorder outcomes were derived from the semistructured Development and Well-Being Assessment interview at 15.5 years of age. The interview is a valid instrument in community and clinical samples.25 International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, Tenth Revision and DSM-IV diagnoses of depressive or any anxiety disorder were generated using a computerized diagnostic algorithm that predicts the likelihood of a clinical diagnostic rating (more information is available at http://www.DAWBA.com).26

We defined individuals as having a depressive disorder or an anxiety disorder if they were categorized in the Development and Well-Being Assessment band predicting at least a 15% probability of clinical diagnosis, a cutoff selected to define the approximate top deciles of the sample. We conducted a series of sensitivity analyses using different phenotype score cut-offs to define binary outcomes and using different measures where available to test the robustness of our findings (eMethods in the Supplement).

Polygenic Risk Score

Construction of the PRS follows the methods described by the International Schizophrenia Consortium.4 The PRS was constructed using results from the second Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (PGC) schizophrenia GWAS (eMethods in the Supplement).1 Polygenic scores were calculated for each ALSPAC individual using the PLINK (version 1.07) score command.27 Scores were created by summing the number of risk alleles present for each SNP (0, 1, or 2) weighted by the logarithm of its odds ratio (OR) for schizophrenia from the PGC.

Our primary analysis used scores generated from a list of SNPs with a GWAS training-set P ≤. 05 threshold, which is the threshold that maximally captures schizophrenia liability.1 Because the composition of a PRS is a balance between true and null effects,28 scores generated using lists of SNPs meeting a series of P value thresholds and using all independent SNPs meeting genome-wide significance as reported by the PGC schizophrenia GWAS1 were used in sensitivity analyses.

Statistical Analysis

Data were analyzed from March 4 to August 13, 2015. We used logistic regression to test the association between outcomes and the schizophrenia PRS. Results are presented as ORs and 95% CIs per SD increase in PRS. Nagelkerke R2 values are also presented as a measure of variance explained. Nonlinear associations between the PRS and outcomes were examined by inclusion of quadratic terms. We examined whether associations with our outcomes were independent by inclusion of all phenotypes within a multivariable model. To correct for multiple testing arising from using different P value thresholds within our sensitivity analyses, we computed permutation-adjusted P values (eMethods in the Supplement).

To test whether the effect size of the schizophrenia PRS was the same or different across phenotypes, we used bivariate probit regression to model pairs of outcomes jointly. We tested equality of regression parameters expressing the effect of the schizophrenia PRS (threshold P = .05) on each outcome using a likelihood ratio test to compare a model that allows effect estimates to differ with a model where the PRS effect was constrained to be equal for both outcomes.29,30 All statistical analyses were performed using STATA statistical software (version 13; StataCorp LP).

Results

Associations Between Schizophrenia PRS and Psychopathological Outcomes

The numbers of individuals who participated in the Psychosis-Like Symptom Interview at 12 and 18 years of age or completed questions relating to negative symptoms at 16.5 years of age and depression and anxiety at 15.5 years of age are shown in Table 1. Of 8230 ALSPAC adolescents whose genetic data passed quality control checks (51.2% male and 48.8% female), 3676 to 5444 participated in assessments from 12 to 18 years of age.

Table 1

Individuals With Outcome Measurea
Age, yData SourceOutcome MeasureBinary Outcome Measure TypeAll ParticipantsGenotyped Participants
No.Outcome, No. (%)No.Outcome, No. (%)
12PLIKSIPsychotic experiencesYes or no6792383 (5.64)5103280 (5.49)
18PLIKSIPsychotic experiencesYes or no4718229 (4.85)3486168 (4.82)
12 and 18bPLIKSIPsychotic experiencesYes or no7452575 (7.72)5444419 (7.70)
16.5CAPENegative symptomsScore <14 or ≥14c5095467 (9.17)3673337 (9.18)
15.5DAWBAdDepression<15% or ≥15% likelihood5365498 (9.28)4106373 (9.08)
15.5DAWBAdAnxiety<15% or ≥15% likelihood5367596 (11.10)4107444 (10.81)

Abbreviations: CAPE, Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences; DAWBA, Development and Well-Being Assessment; PLIKSI, Psychosis-Like Symptom Interview.

aIncludes psychotic experiences, negative symptoms, depression, and anxiety.
bIndicates individuals who completed at least 1 interview session at 12 and/or 18 years of age.
cTotal scores range from 0 to 30, with higher scores indicating worse symptoms.
dAn individual within the 15% band category has a 15% likelihood of having a depressive or anxiety disorder. Higher percentage categories indicate a higher chance of having a depressive or anxiety disorder.

We found no strong evidence that individuals who had a higher PRS, and thus increased genetic risk for schizophrenia, had an increased risk for developing psychotic experiences (OR per SD increase in PRS, 1.08; 95% CI, 0.98-1.19; permutation-adjusted P = .14; R2 = 0.001) (Figure, A, and eTable 2 in the Supplement).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is emss-69048-f001.jpg
Associations Between Polygenic Risk Scores (PRSs) for Schizophrenia and Phenotypes

Odds ratios (ORs) per SD change in PRS for schizophrenia are shown (data markers), with upper and lower error bars indicating 95% CIs. The ORs are shown for a range of PRSs generated using lists of single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) meeting a series of P value thresholds. The bars indicate the log10 times the number of SNPs used to create the PRSs. A genome-wide significant (threshold P = 5 × 10−8) PRS was created from 111 genome-wide significant schizophrenia SNPs as reported by the second Psychiatric Genomics Consortium.1 Red lines indicate the null value.

We observed strong evidence of an association between the schizophrenia PRS and negative symptoms at 16.5 years of age (OR, 1.21; 95% CI, 1.08-1.36; permutation-adjusted P = .001; R2 = 0.007) (Figure, B, and eTable 2 in the Supplement). We also observed strong evidence that individuals with a higher schizophrenia PRS were at an increased risk for anxiety disorder at 15.5 years of age (OR per SD increase in polygenic score, 1.17; 95% CI, 1.06-1.29; permutation-adjusted P = .002; R2 = 0.005) (Figure, D, and eTable 2 in the Supplement). We found no strong evidence of an association between the PRS and depressive disorder at 15.5 years of age (OR per SD increase in polygenic score, 1.02; 95%CI, 0.91-1.13; permutation-adjusted P = .77; R2 = 0.00005) (Figure, C, and eTable 2 in the Supplement).

We found no strong evidence to support nonlinear effects of polygenic risk on any of the phenotypes examined. Results per decile of PRS for a training-set threshold of P = .05 are presented in eTable 3 in the Supplement. Associations with negative symptoms and anxiety disorder were independent and persisted when testing all phenotypes within a multivariable model (eTables 4 to 7 in the Supplement).

Common and Specific Associations With Schizophrenia PRS

Tetrachoric correlations between each psychopathological outcome are shown in eTable 8 in the Supplement. The results of the bivariate analyses examining whether the schizophrenia PRS (threshold P = .05) effect sizes are similar or different across phenotypes are summarized in Table 2. We observed some evidence that the strong associations between the schizophrenia PRS and negative symptoms and anxiety are different from the association between the PRS and depression (both P = .02), with weaker evidence that they are different from the associations of the PRS with psychotic experiences (P = .14 and P = .11, respectively). We found no strong evidence that the association between the PRS effect and negative symptoms differed from that for anxiety (P = .92).

Table 2

Effects of Schizophrenia PRS on Psychopathological Outcomes and Comparison of Specific vs Common Effecta
Phenotype 1Phenotype 2No. of ParticipantsEffect, OR (95% CI)
Phenotype Specific
12CommonP Valueb
Psychotic experiencesNegative symptoms32881.04 (0.93-1.15)1.15 (1.04-1.26)1.09 (1.02-1.18).14
Psychotic experiencesDepressive disorder39651.06 (0.97-1.16)1.02 (0.94-1.12)1.04 (0.97-1.11).58
Psychotic experiencesAnxiety disorder39661.05 (0.96-1.15)1.16 (1.06-1.26)1.11 (1.04-1.19).11
Negative symptomsDepressive disorder28721.17 (1.06-1.30)0.99 (0.90-1.10)1.08 (1.00-1.17).02
Negative symptomsAnxiety disorder28731.17 (1.05-1.30)1.16 (1.05-1.28)1.16 (1.08-1.26).92
Depressive disorderAnxiety disorder41061.01 (0.92-1.10)1.14 (1.04-1.23)1.07 (1.00-1.15).02

Abbreviations: OR, odds ratio; PRS, polygenic risk score.

aIncludes complete case data using bivariate model estimation. The ORs are calculated per SD increase in PRS, with a threshold P = .05 for the discovery sample.
bCalculated from likelihood ratio tests comparing a model assuming psychopathologic-specific effect vs a common effect model when the exposure effect is constrained to be the same across phenotypes. P = .5, for example, would provide little evidence that the association between the PRS and phenotype 1 was different from that for the association between the PRS and phenotype 2. P = .01, for example, would provide strong evidence of a difference. The eMethods in the Supplement details how ORs were derived.

Sensitivity Analyses

Our results were unchanged when we used different cutoff values or different measurement tools to assess our outcomes (eFigures 1-5 in the Supplement), when we adjusted for parental self-reported history of schizophrenia or depression, or when we excluded individuals with a psychotic disorder at 18 years of age. Results were also consistent across all training-set P value thresholds for almost all outcomes. The exception was psychotic experiences, for which we found weak evidence that increased genetic risk was associated with a decreased risk of psychotic experiences at lower training-set P value thresholds (threshold P ≤ 1 × 10−7; OR per SD increase in polygenic score, 0.90; 95% CI, 0.81-0.99; permutation-adjusted P = .04; R2 = 0.002) (Figure, A, and eTable2 in the Supplement).

Discussion

In this study, we examined how an increased genetic risk for schizophrenia is manifest phenotypically during adolescence in the general population. We found no strong evidence of an association with the occurrence of psychotic experiences or depressive disorder. However, we found strong evidence that negative symptoms and anxiety disorders were more common in adolescents with a higher genetic risk and that these risks were independent of each other.

Although the absence of an association between a genetic risk for schizophrenia and psychotic experiences in adolescence may seem surprising, these findings are consistent with those of previous studies that have examined this relationship.17,18 The estimates of association and strength of evidence in this study are very similar to those from a previous study using ALSPAC,18 although the power in our study is substantially greater given the use of a much larger training set to generate the risk-scoring algorithm.

At P value thresholds that maximally capture schizophrenia liability, psychotic experiences were more common in those with higher genetic risk, albeit the CIs included the null. At the most stringent P value thresholds, however, we found weak evidence that genetic risk was associated with reduced psychotic experiences. This finding could be caused by random error or could result from attrition bias. Missing data are likely greater for those who develop a psychotic disorder and for those at high genetic risk. Therefore, psychotic experiences may be underrepresented in participants with high compared with low genetic risk included in these analyses, akin to the apparently protective effect of smoking on Alzheimer disease risk seen using risk rather than rate models of analysis.31 Why this would only be observed at P value thresholds that explain less of the variance for schizophrenia is not clear.

A number of potential explanations exist for our findings of strong evidence of an association between genetic risk for schizophrenia and negative symptoms and anxiety disorder but not with psychotic experiences in the general population. First, genetic risk for schizophrenia may be expressed heterotypically during adolescence as anxiety and negative symptoms, and psychotic experiences may develop later. For the minority of individuals who later develop schizophrenia, anxiety and negative symptoms would represent prodromal symptoms of the disorder. This possibility implies that, compared with psychotic experiences arising later in life, hallucinations and paranoid beliefs arising during adolescence might be explained to a greater degree by nongenetic effects, such as childhood trauma32,33 or cannabis use34, than by genetic risk for schizophrenia. The association between genetic risk for schizophrenia and psychotic experiences might therefore become stronger with increasing age akin to that seen for general cognitive ability.35

Anxiety and negative symptoms might occur as early manifestations of genetic risk for schizophrenia, and we might speculate on possible mechanisms. For example, disturbed biological processes secondary to genetic variation might result in subtle alterations in prediction error processing and attributional salience that might lead to anxiety before the onset of clear-cut psychotic phenomena.36,37 Genetic risk for schizophrenia is also associated with impaired childhood performance IQ in this cohort,38 although we do not know whether variants acting primarily through different brain pathways are differentially related to these phenotypes.

Second, genetic risk for schizophrenia may be expressed during adolescence as increased psychotic experiences, anxiety, and negative symptoms, but psychotic experiences may be observed with greater measurement error; thus, associations would be relatively underpowered for this phenotype. Use of a semistructured interview and similar estimates using questionnaires suggest that this explanation is unlikely but cannot be excluded.

Third, genetic variants identified as showing an association with schizophrenia in the GWAS may only weakly index the risk for hallucinations and delusions and may reflect genetic risk more strongly for other characteristics of the disorder, such as negative symptoms that index severity or chronicity of illness and that might be selected for in clinically ascertained samples. Similarly, such ascertainment might be biased toward those with multiple morbidities, for example, comorbid anxiety disorders. Studies that have examined symptom dimensions within schizophrenia39 and in the general population40,41 show that the heritability of negative and disorganized symptoms is greater than that of positive symptoms and that schizophrenia polygenic risk is more strongly associated with negative and disorganized symptoms than positive symptoms.42

Although evidence exists of genetic overlap between schizophrenia and major depressive disorder in adults,12,43 we found no evidence to support this overlap in our study. Measures of depression in this study may capture more transient disorders in adolescence that obscure a genetic overlap between schizophrenia and a more persistent, chronic form of depression.

Our results indicate that anxiety and negative symptoms are likely to be the best markers of high genetic risk for schizophrenia in population-based samples; however, the variance of these phenotypes explained by schizophrenia genetic risk is small (0.5%-0.7%), and the negative symptoms measure used in the general population might not fully capture the negative symptoms seen in schizophrenia. Our findings have potentially important implications for studies of at-risk samples among whom current approaches for informing prediction of transition rely heavily on psychotic experiences. Our results are consistent with an evolving literature describing anxiety as a common symptom during the prodromal stage of psychosis.4446

Our study has a number of strengths. First, we used the most recent schizophrenia GWAS from the PGC1—the largest schizophrenia data set available—as a training set, thus minimizing measurement error. Second, we used a large, well-characterized, relatively homogeneous, population-based sample for examining psychosis-related phenotypes during adolescence, a key period of development that closely pre-dates the start of the peak in incidence of schizophrenia. Third, we used a semistructured interview to determine the presence of psychotic experiences, as used clinically. We also used multiple measures of depression and anxiety at different ages during adolescence and different cutoffs of these and of measures of psychotic experiences and negative symptoms as sensitivity analyses to test the robustness of our findings.

Our study also has a number of limitations. First, despite the use of one of the largest population-based birth cohorts worldwide with the required data, our sample may not be adequately powered to identify small effects of cumulative genetic risk on the phenotypes examined,19 especially given potential differences in heritability across the phenotypes.14,40,41 Given that our sample is too small to estimate heritability of the phenotypes accurately, determining whether the absence of evidence of association between a phenotype and genetic risk for schizophrenia reflects an absence of genetic correlation or inadequate power is difficult. Second, missing data in the cohort could lead to bias in our estimates. Genetic risk for schizophrenia47 and presence of mental health problems are associated with reduced retention in cohort studies, which would tend to underestimate associations. Although selective missingness may have led to overestimates of association, this possibility seems unlikely for some of our phenotypes but not others. A further limitation is that rare genetic variants are not captured by a GWAS; therefore, we are only able to examine the effect of common variants (as captured by the current GWAS) on adolescent phenotype expression.

Conclusions

Our results highlight the need for GWAS consortia of schizophrenia to include detailed phenotyping data, to examine the extent to which current GWAS findings relate to specific phenotypes, and to identify genetic variants and pathways that are symptom-domain specific rather than to examine the presence of disorder per se. Furthermore, large population-based longitudinal studies with robust measures of these phenotypic constructs are required to determine how genetic risk for schizophrenia is expressed from childhood through adulthood and whether this expression changes with age, to examine potential mediators and moderators of risk, and to determine the usefulness of genetic risk scores for prediction of transition to psychosis. A better understanding of how genetic risk for schizophrenia manifests during development could inform early recognition of problems in those at greatest risk and potentially inform targeted interventions.

Supplementary Material

Supplement

Acknowledgments

Funding/Support: This study was supported by grants G0701503, MR/M006727/1, and MR/K004360/1 from the Medical Research Council, grant 102215/2/13/2 from the Wellcome Trust, and the University of Bristol through core support for the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) and by 23andMe, which supported generation of the genome-wide association data by Sample Logistics and genotyping facilities at the Wellcome Trust Sanger Institute and Laboratory Corporation of America. Drs H. J. Jones, Stergiakouli, Tansey, and Davey Smith are part of the Medical Research Council Integrative Epidemiology Unit at the University of Bristol, which is supported by grant MC_UU_12013/1 from the Medical Research Council and the University of Bristol.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Footnotes

Contributed by

Author Contributions: Drs H. J. Jones and Zammit had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: H. J. Jones, Stergiakouli, Davey Smith, O'Donovan, Owen, Walters, Zammit.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: H. J. Jones, Tansey, Hubbard, Heron, Cannon, Holmans, Lewis, Linden, P. B. Jones, Davey Smith, O'Donovan, Walters, Zammit.

Drafting of the manuscript: H. J. Jones, Cannon, O'Donovan, Zammit.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.

Statistical analysis: H. J. Jones, Stergiakouli, Tansey, Heron, Holmans.

Obtained funding: Cannon, Lewis, Linden, P. B. Jones, Davey Smith, O'Donovan, Owen, Zammit.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Stergiakouli, Lewis, Davey Smith, O'Donovan. Study supervision: Stergiakouli, Heron, Davey Smith, O'Donovan, Zammit.

Additional Contributions: We thank all the families who participated in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting the families, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr O'Donovan received a consultancy fee from Roche in July 2015. No other disclosures were reported.

Contributor Information

Hannah J. Jones, Centre for Academic Mental Health, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, England; Medical Research Council (MRC) Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Bristol, England.

Evie Stergiakouli, Medical Research Council (MRC) Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Bristol, England.

Katherine E. Tansey, Medical Research Council (MRC) Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Bristol, England.

Leon Hubbard, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, Wales.

Jon Heron, Centre for Academic Mental Health, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, England.

Mary Cannon, Department of Psychiatry, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Dublin.

Peter Holmans, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, Wales.

Glyn Lewis, Division of Psychiatry, University College London, London, England.

David E. J. Linden, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, Wales.

Peter B. Jones, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England.

George Davey Smith, Medical Research Council (MRC) Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Bristol, England.

Michael C. O'Donovan, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, Wales.

Michael J. Owen, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, Wales.

James T. Walters, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, Wales.

Stanley Zammit, Centre for Academic Mental Health, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, England; Medical Research Council (MRC) Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Bristol, England.

References

1. Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature. 2014;511(7510):421–427. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
2. Rasic D, Hajek T, Alda M, Uher R. Risk of mental illness in offspring of parents with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of family high-risk studies. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(1):28–38. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
3. Ripke S, O’Dushlaine C, Chambert K, et al. Multicenter Genetic Studies of Schizophrenia Consortium; Psychosis Endophenotypes International Consortium; Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium 2. Genome-wide association analysis identifies 13 new risk loci for schizophrenia. Nat Genet. 2013;45(10):1150–1159. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
4. Purcell SM, Wray NR, Stone JL, et al. International Schizophrenia Consortium. Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature. 2009;460(7256):748–752. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
5. Krapohl E, Euesden J, Zabaneh D, et al. Phenome-wide analysis of genome-wide polygenic scores [published online August 25, 2015] Mol Psychiatry. 10.1038/mp.2015.126. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
6. Buckley PF, Miller BJ, Lehrer DS, Castle DJ. Psychiatric comorbidities and schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35(2):383–402. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
7. Docherty JP, Van Kammen DP, Siris SG, Marder SR. Stages of onset of schizophrenic psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 1978;135(4):420–426. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
8. Tien AY, Eaton WW. Psychopathologic precursors and sociodemographic risk factors for the schizophrenia syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49(1):37–46. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
9. Jones P, Rodgers B, Murray R, Marmot M. Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet. 1994;344(8934):1398–1402. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
10. Turnbull G, Bebbington P. Anxiety and the schizophrenic process: clinical and epidemiological evidence. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2001;36(5):235–243. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
11. Khandaker GM, Barnett JH, White IR, Jones PB. A quantitative meta-analysis of population-based studies of premorbid intelligence and schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2011;132(2-3):220–227. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
12. Lee SH, Ripke S, Neale BM, et al. Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium; International Inflammatory Bowel Disease Genetics Consortium (IIBDGC). Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Nat Genet. 2013;45(9):984–994. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
13. Hamshere ML, Stergiakouli E, Langley K, et al. Shared polygenic contribution between childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and adult schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 2013;203(2):107–111. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
14. Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet. 2013;381(9875):1371–1379. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
15. Demirkan A, Penninx BW, Hek K, et al. Genetic risk profiles for depression and anxiety in adult and elderly cohorts. Mol Psychiatry. 2011;16(7):773–783. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
16. Derks EM, Vorstman JAS, Ripke S, Kahn RS, Ophoff RA, Schizophrenia Psychiatric Genomic Consortium Investigation of the genetic association between quantitative measures of psychosis and schizophrenia: a polygenic risk score analysis. PLoS One. 2012;7(6):e37852. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
17. Sieradzka D, Power RA, Freeman D, et al. Are genetic risk factors for psychosis also associated with dimension-specific psychotic experiences in adolescence? PLoS One. 2014;9(4):e94398. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
18. Zammit S, Hamshere M, Dwyer S, et al. A population-based study of genetic variation and psychotic experiences in adolescents. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(6):1254–1262. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
19. Dudbridge F. Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PLoS Genet. 2013;9(3):e1003348. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
20. Fraser A, Macdonald-Wallis C, Tilling K, et al. Cohort profile: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(1):97–110. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
21. Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, et al. Cohort profile: the “children of the 90s”: the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(1):111–127. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
22. Horwood J, Salvi G, Thomas K, et al. IQ and non-clinical psychotic symptoms in 12-year-olds: results from the ALSPAC birth cohort. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;193(3):185–191. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
23. Zammit S, Kounali D, Cannon M, et al. Psychotic experiences and psychotic disorders at age 18 in relation to psychotic experiences at age 12 in a longitudinal population-based cohort study. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(7):742–750. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
24. Stefanis NC, Hanssen M, Smirnis NK, et al. Evidence that three dimensions of psychosis have a distribution in the general population. Psychol Med. 2002;32(2):347–358. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
25. Goodman R, Ford T, Richards H, Gatward R, Meltzer H. The Development and Well-Being Assessment: description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41(5):645–655. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
26. Goodman A, Heiervang E, Collishaw S, Goodman R. The “DAWBA bands” as an ordered-categorical measure of child mental health: description and validation in British and Norwegian samples. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2011;46(6):521–532. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
27. Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81(3):559–575. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
28. Iyegbe C, Campbell D, Butler A, Ajnakina O, Sham P. The emerging molecular architecture of schizophrenia, polygenic risk scores and the clinical implications for GxE research. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2014;49(2):169–182. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
29. Amemiya T. Qualitative response models: a survey. J Econ Lit. 1981;19(4):1483–1536. [Google Scholar]
30. Kounali D, Zammit S, Wiles N, et al. Common versus psychopathology-specific risk factors for psychotic experiences and depression during adolescence. Psychol Med. 2014;44(12):2557–2566. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
31. Fratiglioni L, Wang HX. Smoking and Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease: review of the epidemiological studies. Behav Brain Res. 2000;113(1-2):117–120. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
32. Bendall S, Jackson HJ, Hulbert CA, McGorry PD. Childhood trauma and psychotic disorders: a systematic, critical review of the evidence. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34(3):568–579. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
33. Kelleher I, Keeley H, Corcoran P, et al. Childhood trauma and psychosis in a prospective cohort study: cause, effect, and directionality. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;70(7):734–741. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
34. Moore THM, Zammit S, Lingford-Hughes A, et al. Cannabis use and risk of psychotic or affective mental health outcomes: a systematic review. Lancet. 2007;370(9584):319–328. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
35. McIntosh AM, Gow A, Luciano M, et al. Polygenic risk for schizophrenia is associated with cognitive change between childhood and old age. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73(10):938–943. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
36. Heinz A. Dopaminergic dysfunction in alcoholism and schizophrenia: psychopathological and behavioral correlates. Eur Psychiatry. 2002;17(1):9–16. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
37. Heinz A, Schlagenhauf F. Dopaminergic dysfunction in schizophrenia: salience attribution revisited. Schizophr Bull. 2010;36(3):472–485. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
38. Hubbard L, Tansey KE, Rai D, et al. Evidence of common genetic overlap between schizophrenia and cognition. Schizophr Bull. published online December 16, 2015. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
39. Dworkin RH, Lenzenweger MF, Moldin SO, Skillings GF, Levick SE. A multidimensional approach to the genetics of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145(9):1077–1083. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
40. Zavos HM, Freeman D, Haworth CM, et al. Consistent etiology of severe, frequent psychotic experiences and milder, less frequent manifestations: a twin study of specific psychotic experiences in adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(9):1049–1057. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
41. Sieradzka D, Power RA, Freeman D, Cardno AG, Dudbridge F, Ronald A. Heritability of individual psychotic experiences captured by common genetic variants in a community sample of adolescents. Behav Genet. 2015;45(5):493–502. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
42. Fanous AH, Zhou B, Aggen SH, et al. Schizophrenia Psychiatric Genome-Wide Association Study (GWAS) Consortium. Genome-wide association study of clinical dimensions of schizophrenia: polygenic effect on disorganized symptoms. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169(12):1309–1317. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
43. Schulze TG, Akula N, Breuer R, et al. Bipolar Genome Study. Molecular genetic overlap in bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and major depressive disorder. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2014;15(3):200–208. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
44. Pallanti S, Cantisani A, Grassi G. Anxiety as a core aspect of schizophrenia. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2013;15(5):354. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
45. Fusar-Poli P, Nelson B, Valmaggia L, Yung AR, McGuire PK. Comorbid depressive and anxiety disorders in 509 individuals with an at-risk mental state: impact on psychopathology and transition to psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(1):120–131. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
46. Fusar-Poli P, Yung AR, McGorry P, van Os J. Lessons learned from the psychosis high-risk state: towards a general staging model of prodromal intervention. Psychol Med. 2014;44(1):17–24. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
47. Martin J, Tilling K, Hubbard L, et al. Genetic risk for schizophrenia associated with non-participation over time in a population-based cohort study. Am J Epidemiol. In press. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]

Citations & impact 


Impact metrics

Jump to Citations

Citations of article over time

Alternative metrics

Altmetric item for https://www.altmetric.com/details/5049845
Altmetric
Discover the attention surrounding your research
https://www.altmetric.com/details/5049845

Article citations


Go to all (127) article citations

Data 


Data behind the article

This data has been text mined from the article, or deposited into data resources.

Similar Articles 


To arrive at the top five similar articles we use a word-weighted algorithm to compare words from the Title and Abstract of each citation.


Funding 


Funders who supported this work.

Medical Research Council (11)

Wellcome Trust (3)