Abstract
Background
Although habituation is one of the most ancient and fundamental forms of learning, its regulators and its relevance for human disease are poorly understood.Methods
We manipulated the orthologs of 286 genes implicated in intellectual disability (ID) with or without comorbid autism spectrum disorder (ASD) specifically in Drosophila neurons, and we tested these models in light-off jump habituation. We dissected neuronal substrates underlying the identified habituation deficits and integrated genotype-phenotype annotations, gene ontologies, and interaction networks to determine the clinical features and molecular processes that are associated with habituation deficits.Results
We identified >100 genes required for habituation learning. For 93 of these genes, a role in habituation learning was previously unknown. These genes characterize ID disorders with macrocephaly and/or overgrowth and comorbid ASD. Moreover, individuals with ASD from the Simons Simplex Collection carrying damaging de novo mutations in these genes exhibit increased aberrant behaviors associated with inappropriate, stereotypic speech. At the molecular level, ID genes required for normal habituation are enriched in synaptic function and converge on Ras/mitogen-activated protein kinase (Ras/MAPK) signaling. Both increased Ras/MAPK signaling in gamma-aminobutyric acidergic (GABAergic) neurons and decreased Ras/MAPK signaling in cholinergic neurons specifically inhibit the adaptive habituation response.Conclusions
Our work supports the relevance of habituation learning to ASD, identifies an unprecedented number of novel habituation players, supports an emerging role for inhibitory neurons in habituation, and reveals an opposing, circuit-level-based mechanism for Ras/MAPK signaling. These findings establish habituation as a possible, widely applicable functional readout and target for pharmacologic intervention in ID/ASD.Free full text
Habituation Learning is a Widely Affected Mechanism in Drosophila Models of Intellectual Disability and Autism Spectrum Disorders
Abstract
Background:
Although habituation is one of the most ancient and fundamental forms of learning, its regulators and relevance for human disease are poorly understood.
Methods:
We manipulated the orthologs of 286 genes implicated in intellectual disability (ID) with or without comorbid autism spectrum disorder (ASD) specifically in Drosophila neurons, and tested these models in light-off jump habituation. We dissected neuronal substrates underlying the identified habituation deficits and integrated genotype-phenotype annotations, gene ontologies and interaction networks to determine the clinical features and molecular processes that are associated with habituation deficits.
Results:
We identified more than 100 genes required for habituation learning. For the vast majority of these, 93 genes, a role in habituation learning was previously unknown. These genes characterize ID disorders with macrocephaly/overgrowth and comorbid ASD. Moreover, ASD individuals from the Simons Simplex Collection (SSC) carrying damaging de novo mutations in these genes exhibit increased aberrant behaviors associated with inappropriate, stereotypic speech. At the molecular level, ID genes required for normal habituation are enriched in synaptic function and converge on Ras-MAPK signaling. Both increased Ras-MAPK signaling in GABAergic and decreased Ras-MAPK signaling in cholinergic neurons specifically inhibit the adaptive habituation response.
Conclusions:
Our work supports the relevance of habituation learning to autism, identifies an unprecedented number of novel habituation players, supports an emerging role for inhibitory neurons in habituation and reveals an opposing, circuit-level-based mechanism for Ras-MAPK signaling. This establishes habituation as a possible, widely applicable functional readout and target for pharmacologic intervention in ID/ASD.
Introduction:
Habituation is one of the most ancient and fundamental forms of learning, conserved across the animal kingdom (1). It causes an organism’s initial response to repeated meaningless stimuli to gradually decline. Learning to ignore irrelevant stimuli as a result of habituation is thought to represent a filter mechanism that prevents information overload, allowing for selective attention, thereby focusing cognitive resources on relevant matters. Habituation learning has been proposed to represent an important prerequisite for higher cognitive functions (2–4). In line with this, habituation in infants correlates better than other measures with later cognitive abilities (5). However, key players and molecular mechanisms underlying habituation are poorly understood (6).
In humans, deficits in habituation have been reported in a number of neuropsychiatric and behavioral disorders. In particular, defective cortical filtering of sensory stimuli and information overload, as expected to arise from habituation deficits, are thought to represent mechanisms contributing to autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (7, 8). A decreased ability to habituate has been described in a fraction of ASD individuals (9–11), but has not been connected yet to specific genetic defects, with a single exception. Recently, two independent studies demonstrated habituation deficits in patients with Fragile X syndrome, the most common monogenic cause of intellectual disability (ID) and ASD (12, 13), confirming previously reported habituation deficits in Fmr1 KO mice (14, 15). Habituation deficits have also been reported in a limited number of other ID or ASD (ID/ASD) disease models (16–19).
Because assessing human gene function in habituation is challenging, we utilized a cross-species approach. We apply light-off jump habituation in Drosophila to increase our knowledge on the genetic control of habituation and, at the same time, to address the relevance of decreased habituation in ID and in comorbid ASD disorders. Since ID is present in 70% of individuals with ASD (20), monogenic causes of ID provide a unique molecular windows to ASD pathology (21). Drosophila is a powerful, well-established model for ID (22–24) and offers genome-wide resources to study gene function in large scale (25, 26). Several forms of habituation have been established in Drosophila (27–31). Deficits in light-off jump habituation have already been reported in several ID models (23, 32–36) and in classical learning and memory mutants (28, 31). Moreover, this form of habituation can be assessed in a high-throughput manner. In the light-off jump paradigm, the initial jump response to repeated light-off stimuli gradually wanes, as has been demonstrated not due to sensory adaptation (a decrease in detecting the stimulus) or motor fatigue (a decrease in the ability to execute the response) but as a result of learned adaptation of the startle circuit (31). This behavior meets all habituation criteria (37), including spontaneous recovery and dishabituation with a novel stimulus (31, 38).
Here, we use inducible RNA interference (RNAi) in Drosophila to systematically assess the role of Drosophila orthologs of 286 genes that are well-established to cause ID in humans when mutated (hereinafter referred to as ID genes). 68 of them (20%) have also been implicated in ASD (39, 40) (Table S1), hereinafter referred to as ID plus ASD-associated genes.
Methods and Materials
Investigated ID genes
A systematic source of ID genes and their Drosophila orthologs is available online (SysID database, sysid.cmbi.umcn.nl (41)). We investigated the Drosophila orthologs of 286 human ID genes from the SysID category primary ID genes (Table S1) (containing mutations with robust published evidence for causality, see Supplemental Methods (SM)). SysID inclusion criteria and in/exclusion criteria of experimentally investigated genes are indicated in the SM). In brief, the vast majority of genes are from the first data freeze of the SysID database (status of mid 2010). Genes have been included based on conservation in Drosophila, available tools (RNAi) from large-scale resources and viability as a prerequisite for behavioral testing. No selection was performed.
Light-off jump habituation assay
3- to 7-day-old flies were subjected to the light-off jump habituation paradigm in two independent 16-unit light-off jump systems (manufactured and distributed by Aktogen Ltd.). After 5 min adaption, flies were simultaneously exposed to a series of 100 light-off pulses (15 ms) with 1 s interval. The noise amplitude of wing vibration during jump responses was recorded. An appropriate threshold (0.8 V) was applied to filter out background noise. Data were collected by a custom-made Labview Software (National Instruments). Flies were considered as habituated when not jumping in five consecutive light-off trials (no-jump criterion). Habituation was quantified as the number of trials required to reach the no-jump criterion (Trials To Criterion (TTC)).
Information about the identification of Drosophila orthologs, proposed disease mechanism, Drosophila stocks, phenotype reproducibility, validation of the automated jump scoring and of jump specificity, fatigue assay, quality criteria for RNAi lines, annotation of ID plus ASD associated genes, enrichment analysis, comparison of behavior and cognition in ASD individuals from the SSC, molecular interaction network, clustering, physical interaction enrichment (PIE), data visualization and statistics are described in the SM.
Results:
Systematic identification of habituation deficits in Drosophila models of ID
To identify novel genes implicated in habituation, we systematically investigated the role of 278 Drosophila orthologs representing 286 human ID genes in the light-off jump habituation paradigm. We induced neuron-specific knockdowns of each ID gene ortholog by RNAi (25) using 513 RNAi lines fulfilling previously established quality criteria (41, 42), with two independent constructs per gene whenever available. These were crossed to the panneuronal elav-Gal4 driver line (see SM). Knockdown is a suitable approach for modeling of the here-investigated human disease conditions since (partial) loss of function is considered to be the underlying mechanism in the vast majority of these disorders (42) (Table S1). Restricting gene knockdown to neurons eliminates potential effects on viability or behavioral performance originating from an essential role of genes in other tissues and establishes neuron-autonomous mechanisms.
Knockdown and control flies of identical genetic background were subjected to a series of 100 light-off stimuli, hereinafter referred to as trials, in the light-off jump habituation paradigm. The screening procedure and paradigm allowed us to distinguish the following parameters: viability, initial jump response (percentage of flies that jumped in at least one of the first five trials), and premature and reduced habituation, with the latter representing the learning-defective phenotype category of main interest. Genotypes with an initial jump response ≥50% but premature habituation were subjected to a secondary assay to exclude fatigue as a confounder of premature habituation (see SM, Table S2 and Figure S4). Based on these parameters, genes were assigned to at least one of four phenotype categories (Figure 1A): (1) “not affected”: (both) tested RNAi lines targeting such genes were viable, showed good initial jump response, and had no significant effect on habituation (based on the FDR-corrected p-value (padj), see SM); (2) “non-performers”: at least one RNAi line led to lethality, poor jump response (<50% initial jumpers), or premature habituation because of increased fatigue; (3) “habituation deficient”: at least one RNAi line showed good initial jump response but failed to suppress the response with the increasing number of light-off trials (based on padj); and (4) “premature habituation”: at least one RNAi line showed good initial jump response followed by faster decline (based on padj), without fatigue being detectable in the secondary assay. Still, this latter phenotype category can result from other defects than improved habituation, and will be investigated elsewhere. In this study we focus on habituation deficits (3), corresponding to the phenotype that has been shown in ID and ASD (9–13).
We validated the experimental approach to identify genes which, if manipulated, cause habituation deficits (hereinafter referred to as habituation deficient genes) by recapitulating published habituation deficits of Drosophila ID null mutant models G9a (23) and Synapsin (43), and of the classical learning and memory mutant dunce (28, 44, 45) (Figure 1B,,CC,,D).D). This demonstrated that light-off jump habituation upon RNAi can efficiently identify genetic regulators of habituation learning. We also validated the technical accuracy of the automated jump scoring methodology by comparing automated and manually assessed jumping of controls and a number of ID models (SM, Figure S1).
In our screen, we found that the Drosophila orthologs of 98 human ID genes (35% of all investigated orthologs) are required, in neurons, for habituation learning. This phenotype represents a highly specific defect in behavioral adaptation to the stimulus; flies keep on jumping in response to the repetitive light-off stimulus, illustrating that they do not suffer from broad neuronal transmission deficits (which would disable jumping), fatigue, sensory or other deficiencies. No excessive locomotion was observed when handling the flies, and no stimulus hypersensitivity or random jumping was found (see SM and Figure S2, S3 for validation of light-off jump habituation assay specificity). 27% of ID gene orthologs had no effect on habituation, 41% fell into the category of “non-performers”, and 8% showed “premature habituation” without detectable fatigue. The complete list of habituation screen results and distribution of human ID genes in phenotype categories can be found in Table S2, S3. The screen thus identified nearly a hundred orthologs of disease genes controlling habituation learning.
Habituation deficits characterize ID genes with synaptic function
We first asked whether genes characterized by habituation deficits in Drosophila converge on specific biological process. ID genes are known to be enriched in a number of biological processes, but which are important for habituation? Performing an enrichment analysis of ID-enriched Gene Ontology-based (GO) categories (see SM) against the background of the investigated ID genes, we found that “habituation deficient” genes are significantly enriched in a sole GO-based category: processes related to the synapse (22/44 ID genes, E=1.59, p=0.024, Figure 2, Table S4). No enriched GO terms were found in the “not affected” category. Together, our results support synaptic processes to be crucial for habituation, as previously shown for other forms of this behavior (46, 47).
Drosophila habituation deficits characterize ID genes associated with macrocephaly
To understand whether habituation deficits in Drosophila represent a proxy of specific phenotypes in human individuals, we performed enrichment analysis among ID-associated clinical features (41). We found that orthologs of ID genes characterized by habituation deficits in Drosophila are specifically enriched among ID genes associated with macrocephaly/overgrowth (Figure 3, E=2.19, p=0.018, Table S4). In contrast, ID genes characterized as “non-performers” show enrichment in different, severe ID-associated features such as endocrine, limb and eye anomalies, brain malformations and obesity (Figure S5, Table S4). Moreover, ID genes not giving rise to habituation deficits (“not affected” category) did not show any enrichment among ID-associated clinical features (Figure 3, Table S4).
Habituation deficits characterize ID genes associated with ASD and deficits in specific ASD-relevant behavioral domains
There is a long-known relationship between macrocephaly and autism (48). For this reason and because of the potential relevance of habituation deficits to ASD (9–11), we decided to further investigate the relationship of Drosophila habituation and human ASD. We used the Simons Simplex Collection (SSC) (40), a genetically and phenotypically well-characterized cohort of sporadic ASD individuals. We matched genes with likely gene-disrupting (LGD) and likely damaging de novo mutations (49, 50) in this ASD cohort to those included in our experimental Drosophila habituation approach. 47 ASD individuals carried mutations in 33 of the investigated genes (Table S5). We first asked whether these ID plus ASD-associated genes preferentially fall into a specific Drosophila phenotype category. They are significantly enriched among the genes that in Drosophila caused habituation deficits (Figure 4A, E=1.64, p=0.029, Table S4, ASD SSC). Independently, significant enrichment was obtained for high-confidence ID plus ASD-associated genes identified from the SFARI database (39) (38 investigated genes, Figure 4B, E=1.65, p=0.016, Table S4, ASD SFARI), suggesting a relationship between Drosophila habituation deficits and human ASD.
To further characterize the relationship between Drosophila habituation and human phenotypes, we divided the SSC individuals into two distinct clusters based on their habituation phenotype in the corresponding fly models: habituation deficits (N=22 individuals, 17 genes) and no habituation deficits (N=12 individuals, 9 genes) (Table S5; another N=13 individuals, 7 genes fall into the non-informative phenotype groups “non-performers”/“premature habituation”). We compared both groups across five broad quantitative measures of behavior and cognition: cognitive ability (full-scale IQ); Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS); depression and anxiety (Child Behavior Checklist Internalizing Disorders, CBCL-Int); impulsivity, attention and conduct (Child Behavior Checklist Externalizing Disorders, CBCL-Ext); and atypical behavior (Aberrant Behavior Checklist, ABC). There was no significant difference for IQ (p=0.61), SRS (p=0.62), CBCL-Int (p=0.59) or CBCL-Ext (p=0.37), but a trend for ABC (p=0.04; Figure 4C, Table S6). This effect is mainly driven by the ABC subdomain of inappropriate, stereotypic speech (p=0.0003), not from the subdomains of irritability (p=0.1), hyperactivity (p=0.86), lethargy (p=0.54) or stereotypy (p=0.91) (Table S6). In summary, these data indicate that habituation deficits in Drosophila are relevant to ASD-implicated genes. They also suggest that SSC individuals carrying de novo mutations in genes associated with habituation deficits in Drosophila show a higher rate and/or severity of atypical behaviors associated with inappropriate and stereotypic speech.
Molecular networks and modules underlying habituation
With the rich repertoire of nearly a hundred genes required for habituation that moreover show specificity for ASD and synapse function, we set out to determine the molecular pathways these genes are operating in. ID gene products are significantly interconnected via protein-protein interactions (51, 52). Consistent with previously published findings (41), ID genes investigated in our screen are 1.69 times enriched in interactions compared to 1000 randomly chosen protein sets of the same size and number of known interactions (physical interaction enrichment (PIE) score (53) =1.69; p<0.001). To identify biologically relevant modules, we resolved this network into communities with even tighter interconnectivity using unsupervised community clustering (54). This analysis resulted in 26 communities containing 109 proteins (Figure 5A, Table S7). Their proximity and specificity for ID-enriched GO-based processes are depicted in Figure S6. Mapping “habituation deficient” genes onto the communities (Figure 5A, red circles) highlighted modules with high incidence of habituation deficits (Figure 5A).
A key role for ID and ASD-associated Ras signaling in habituation
Five communities form a large, interconnected module with high incidences of habituation deficits. However, the tightly interconnected hub at its center is characterized by the absence of habituation deficits (Figure 5A, square). This hub represents the key proteins of Ras-MAPK signaling (Figure 5B). This pathway, best known for its role in cancer, underlies a group of disorders collectively referred as Rasopathies. Importantly, while 92% of the modeled ID disorders are thought to result from loss of function of the underlying genes, Rasopathies are caused by gain-of-function mutations in the core pathway (Figure 5C, Table S1). The utilized RNAi approach, despite addressing gene function, did thus not recapitulate the molecular pathology of these specific cognitive disorders. However, Rasopathies can also result from loss of function in negative regulators of the pathway. We therefore asked whether the same genetic mechanisms that cause Rasopathies in humans also hold true for habituation deficits in Drosophila. In our screen, we tested habituation of two negative regulators of Ras: NF1 (Drosophila Nf1) (55) and SPRED1 (Drosophila Spred) (56, 57). Panneuronal knockdown of either regulator caused strong habituation deficits (Figure 5D, in red). We therefore tested a constitutively active Ras mutant, Ras1R68Q (58). Heterozygous Ras1R68Q flies showed strong habituation deficits compared to the control flies with the same genetic background (p=3.56x10−9; Figure 5D, in green). The same was true when we overexpressed, specifically in neurons, Ras1R68Q allele from an inducible transgene (p=1.96x10−6; Figure 5D, in green). We conclude that increased activity of Ras, causing Rasopathies and associated cognitive deficits in humans, causes habituation deficits in Drosophila.
Habituation-inhibiting function of increased Ras-MAPK signaling maps to inhibitory/GABAergic neurons
We next aimed to identify in which type of neurons the habituation-inhibiting function of Ras-MAPK signaling resides. Because the well-characterized neurons of the giant fiber circuit controlling the light-off jump response are cholinergic (59), just as the majority of excitatory neurons in Drosophila, we first tested whether increased Ras-MAPK signaling activity would induce habituation deficits when directed to cholinergic neurons. For this, we adopted the knockdown of negative Ras regulators (Nf1, Spred), expressed constitutively active Ras1 (Ras1R68Q), and tested expression of a gain-of-function allele of Raf (RafGOF), a downstream mediator of Ras signaling. None of these, when driven by the cholinergic Cha-Gal4 driver, recapitulated the panneuronally evoked habituation deficits (Figure 6A).
Because of the recently established role of GABAergic neurons in Drosophila olfactory and proboscis extension reflex habituation (29, 60, 61) and the emerging importance of GABA inhibition in autism (62), we next targeted GABA neurons using the Gad1-Gal4 driver and the same toolbox. This consistently induced habituation deficits in all tested conditions (Figure 6B). We conclude that the habituation-inhibiting function of increased Ras-MAPK signaling maps to GABAergic neurons.
Ras-MAPK signaling in cholinergic neurons is essential for habituation learning
Impaired jump response/increased fatigue associated with Ras, Raf and Mek knockdown in the screen could potentially mask an essential role for Ras signaling in habituation, in addition to the habituation-inhibiting function of increased Ras-MAPK signaling. In fact, our screen also identified habituation deficits upon RNAi of the positive Ras-MAPK regulators Sos and Csw. We therefore downregulated Ras-MAPK activity by crossing the UAS-based RNAi lines targeting Sos and Csw, but also RNAi lines targeting Ras, Raf and Mek, to the GABAergic driver Gad1-Gal4. We did not observe any detrimental effect on habituation (Figure 6D). In contrast, downregulating Ras-MAPK signaling in cholinergic neurons consistently prevented normal habituation learning (Figure 6C). We conclude that Ras-MAPK signaling is essential in cholinergic but not in GABAergic neurons. Thus, Ras-MAPK signaling plays a dual, opposing role in inhibitory versus excitatory neurons in habituation learning.
Discussion:
Drosophila screen demonstrates that genes implicated in ASD are important for habituation learning
To systematically address the genetic basis of habituation deficits associated with neurodevelopmental disorders, we investigated 286 ID genes with a clear Drosophila ortholog in light-off jump habituation. Panneuronal knockdown of the orthologs of 98 ID genes specifically suppressed the adaptive habituation response to repeated stimulation without affecting organismal health or jump ability. Follow-up work on the Ras-MAPK pathway raised this number to 104. 93 of these are novel regulators of habituation, substantially exceeding the sum of previously known regulators of habituation across species and paradigms. Stringent criteria for RNAi specificity and correction for multiple testing (see SM) in our experiments ensured a minimal level of potential false positive discoveries. Of thirteen previously identified ID genes with habituation deficits, our screen confirmed ten (Table S8). Our approach and data, although based on experiments in another species, suggest that deficits in habituation learning are a widely affected mechanism in ID. Habituation deficits might be a hallmark of even more ID genes than determined here. In particular, the phenotype category of “non-performers” is likely to contain genes with promiscuous functions masking a specific role in habituation learning.
Enrichment analysis of ID-associated clinical features revealed that “habituation deficient” ID genes are preferentially characterized by macrocephaly/overgrowth, associated for long with ASD (48). Strikingly, we found that mutations in genes associated with Drosophila habituation deficits are significantly overrepresented among ID genes that are also implicated in ASD (52% (SSC cohort); 53% (SFARI database)). In comparison the frequency of habituation deficits among ID genes not associated with ASD is 24%. SSC individuals carrying mutations in these genes show a high rate and/or severity of aberrant behaviors associated with stereotypic speech. Habituation deficits thus represent a common phenotypic signature of ASD in Drosophila and highlight specific behavioral subdomains affected in ASD. Future work has to establish whether habituation deficits are a direct basis for these clinical features, or are one of many factors involved.
Synapse-related processes and Ras-MAPK signaling play a key role in habituation
Synapse biology has been proposed to play a central role in ASD (63). Our data show that among the investigated disease genes, “habituation deficient” genes are specifically enriched in genes with synaptic function. This is in line with habituation representing a measurable form of synaptic plasticity (7, 47, 64).
Analyzing the distribution of “habituation deficient” genes in ID-specific molecular interaction networks, we discovered that they accumulate in a multiple-community module and connect to the Ras-MAPK pathway core proteins Ras, Raf and Mek (Figure 5A,,B).B). We observed habituation deficits upon panneuronal knockdown of Ras negative regulators and panneuronal expression of the constitutively active Ras allele Ras1R68Q (Figure 5C), demonstrating that increased Ras-mediated signaling causes habituation deficits. Moreover, proteins encoded by “habituation deficient” genes form a significantly interconnected module (Figure 7). The coherence of this module further supports the validity of the chosen RNAi approach to identify genes and molecular processes regulating habituation learning. The module contains a number of synaptic proteins (Figure 7) with not yet investigated roles in Ras signaling. It would be interesting to determine whether some of these enlarge the spectrum of diseases caused by deregulated Ras signaling.
Ras-MAPK signaling exerts a dual but opposing role in inhibitory versus excitatory neurons, a novel systems-level mechanism
Identification of neuronal substrates in which specific ID genes are required to warrant habituation learning is important fundamental problem. Restoring the function of affected neurons might also represent a suitable treatment strategy. The light-off jump startle circuit of Drosophila is relatively simple and its cholinergic nature is well described (59). However, it is not known how habituation of this circuit is regulated. The commonly accepted view regards synaptic depression in excitatory neurons, induced by repetitive stimulation, as the underlying mechanism (46, 65). This has recently been challenged by Ramaswami and colleagues who showed that plasticity of inhibitory, GABAergic neurons drives two non-startle types of habituation (60, 61). We found that increased activity of our identified key pathway, Ras-MAPK, in GABAergic but not in cholinergic neurons causes deficits in light-off jump habituation. Our results thus support inhibitory circuits as crucial components of habituation learning across different paradigms and sensory modalities. Further experiments are needed to establish the direct involvement of GABAergic signaling. At the same time, we identified that also decreased Ras-MAPK signaling activity can lead to habituation deficits. Yet, the neuronal substrates of these deficits are different and map to excitatory, cholinergic neurons. Although our experiments do not distinguish between developmental effects and acute circuit plasticity, the opposing role for Ras-MAPK signaling on habituation may provide new insights into mechanisms of neural plasticity in health and disease. It may also have crucial implications for treatment of Rasopathies. Future clinical trials, as opposed to those that broadly decreased Ras activity and failed (66), may need more attention towards restoring circuit function and balance.
Translational value and application of cross-species habituation measures for diagnosis and treatment of ID and ASD
Based on our findings that habituation is widely affected in Drosophila models of ID, and that habituation deficits are particularly common among genes also implicated in ASD, we propose that disrupted habituation may be one of the mechanisms that contribute to ID/ASD pathology.
The emerging importance of inhibitory inputs for habituation ((29, 60) and this study) and sensory information filtering in the cortical centers of the brain (67, 68) suggests the existence of an overarching circuit-based mechanism responsible for prevention of inappropriate behavioral responses (7). Though our findings that habituation deficits in Drosophila correlate with increased rate and/or severity of atypical ASD-related behaviors compared to ID genes without habituation deficits should be replicated, we speculate that disrupted habituation arising from GABAergic defects may contribute to these ASD features. If future work can establish a substantial contribution of deficits in habituation learning to patient outcomes, cross-species habituation could become an attractive mechanism-specific functional readout—a pressing need for efficient personalized (pharmacological) treatment in the field of neurodevelopmental disorders. Implementing suitable low-burden protocols for habituation measures in clinical research and diagnostics of ID/ASD, such as those developed for investigation of habituation deficits in Fragile X syndrome (12, 13), will help to further delineate the affected cognitive domains that may correlate with or arise from deficient habituation. In future clinical trials, these could serve as objective and quantitative readouts for patient stratification in mechanism-based treatment strategies and for monitoring of drug efficacy. Dissection of the underlying defective mechanisms in Drosophila can at the same time identify novel targets for treatment, with high-throughput light-off jump habituation serving as a translational pipeline for drug testing.
Supplementary Material
1
2
3
4
5
6
Acknowledgements:
We thank Dr Erika Virágh and Enikő Csapó (Biological Research Centre, Szeged, Hungary), moreover Dr Judit Bíró and Márk Péter-Szabó (Voalaz Ltd., Szeged, Hungary) for their contribution to the validation of the Drosophila semi-automated light-off jump reflex habituation paradigm. We acknowledge the Vienna Drosophila Resource Center and Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center (NIH P40OD018537) for providing Drosophila strains. We thank the anonymous expert referees for constructive feedback. This research was supported in part by the European Union’s FP7 TACTICS, OPTIMISTIC, Aggressotype and MATRICS (HEALTH grant agreement numbers n°278948, n°305697, n°602805 and n°603016) to J.C.G., by a grant from the National Science Foundation (CBET-1747506) to C.R.v.R., by the FP7 large-scale integrated network Gencodys (HEALTH-241995) to Z.A. and A.S., by a TOP grant (912-12-109) from The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO), by a Horizon 2020 Marie Sklodowska-Curie European Training Network grant (MiND, 643051), by a grant from the Jérôme Léjeune foundation, and by a grant awarded under the Australian National Health & Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Centre for Research Excellence Scheme (APP1117394) to A.S., and by U.S. National Institute for Mental Health (NIMH) funding (R01MH101221 to E.E.E. & R01MH100047 to R.B.). E.E.E is an investigator of the Howard Hughes Medical Institute. We are grateful to all of the families at the participating Simons Simplex Collection (SSC) sites, as well as the principal investigators (A. Beaudet, R. Bernier, J. Constantino, E. Cook, E. Fombonne, D. Geschwind, R. Goin-Kochel, E. Hanson, D. Grice, A. Klin, D. Ledbetter, C. Lord, C. Martin, D. Martin, R. Maxim, J. Miles, O. Ousley, K. Pelphrey, B. Peterson, J. Piggot, C. Saulnier, M. State, W. Stone, J. Sutcliffe, C. Walsh, Z. Warren, E. Wijsman). We appreciate obtaining access to phenotypic data on SFARI Base. Approved researchers can obtain the SSC population dataset described in this study (http://sfari.org/resources/simons-simplex-collection) by applying at https://base.sfari.org.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Financial disclosures:
In the past 3 years, J.C.G. has acted as a consultant to Boehringer Ingelheim GmbH but is not an employee, stock- or share-holder of this company. E.E.E. is on the scientific advisory board (SAB) of DNAnexus, Inc.. Z.A. is a director and shareholder of Aktogen Ltd.. L.A. is a director of Aktogen Ltd.. The commercial light-off jump habituation system was purchased from Aktogen Ltd.. Aktogen Ltd. provided training of the personnel and ~ 150 experiments from the initial screen were performed at Aktogen Ltd. by M.F. and L.A.. All other authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
References:
Full text links
Read article at publisher's site: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.04.029
Read article for free, from open access legal sources, via Unpaywall: http://www.biologicalpsychiatryjournal.com/article/S0006322319313265/pdf
Citations & impact
Impact metrics
Citations of article over time
Alternative metrics
Article citations
KDM5-mediated transcriptional activation of ribosomal protein genes alters translation efficiency to regulate mitochondrial metabolism in neurons.
Nucleic Acids Res, 52(11):6201-6219, 01 Jun 2024
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 38597673 | PMCID: PMC11194071
Drosophila Contributions towards Understanding Neurofibromatosis 1.
Cells, 13(8):721, 21 Apr 2024
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 38667335 | PMCID: PMC11048932
Review Free full text in Europe PMC
The ortholog of human ssDNA-binding protein SSBP3 influences neurodevelopment and autism-like behaviors in Drosophila melanogaster.
PLoS Biol, 21(7):e3002210, 24 Jul 2023
Cited by: 0 articles | PMID: 37486945 | PMCID: PMC10399856
Strategies for dissecting the complexity of neurodevelopmental disorders.
Trends Genet, 40(2):187-202, 08 Nov 2023
Cited by: 4 articles | PMID: 37949722
Review
Evidence for Prepulse Inhibition of Visually Evoked Motor Response in Drosophila melanogaster.
Biology (Basel), 12(4):635, 21 Apr 2023
Cited by: 2 articles | PMID: 37106835 | PMCID: PMC10135638
Go to all (23) article citations
Data
Data behind the article
This data has been text mined from the article, or deposited into data resources.
BioStudies: supplemental material and supporting data
Similar Articles
To arrive at the top five similar articles we use a word-weighted algorithm to compare words from the Title and Abstract of each citation.
Disruption of POGZ Is Associated with Intellectual Disability and Autism Spectrum Disorders.
Am J Hum Genet, 98(3):541-552, 01 Mar 2016
Cited by: 92 articles | PMID: 26942287 | PMCID: PMC4890241
De novo loss-of-function mutations in WAC cause a recognizable intellectual disability syndrome and learning deficits in Drosophila.
Eur J Hum Genet, 24(8):1145-1153, 13 Jan 2016
Cited by: 20 articles | PMID: 26757981 | PMCID: PMC4970694
Systematic phenomics analysis of autism-associated genes reveals parallel networks underlying reversible impairments in habituation.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 117(1):656-667, 21 Nov 2019
Cited by: 41 articles | PMID: 31754030 | PMCID: PMC6968627
Translating the Role of mTOR- and RAS-Associated Signalopathies in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Models, Mechanisms and Treatment.
Genes (Basel), 12(11):1746, 30 Oct 2021
Cited by: 5 articles | PMID: 34828352 | PMCID: PMC8624393
Review Free full text in Europe PMC
Funding
Funders who supported this work.
Dutch Research Council (NWO)
Howard Hughes Medical Institute
NICHD NIH HHS (2)
Grant ID: P50 HD103524
Grant ID: U54 HD083091
NIH HHS (1)
Grant ID: P40 OD018537
NIMH NIH HHS (2)
Grant ID: R01 MH101221
Grant ID: R01 MH100047