This is the html version of the file http://www.lscp.net/persons/dupoux/teaching/JOURNEE_AUTOMNE_CogMaster_2014-15/readings_neuromyths/Howard-Jones_et_al_(2009).Neuroscience_litteracy.pdf.
Google automatically generates html versions of documents as we crawl the web.
The Neuroscience Literacy of Trainee Teachers
Page 1
Neuroscience Literacy   1 
Running head: Neuroscience Literacy
The Neuroscience Literacy of Trainee
Teachers
Paul Howard-Jones, Lorna Franey, Rasha Mashmoushi
and Yen-Chun Liao
Graduate School of Education, University of Bristol
Paper presented at the British Educational Research Association Annual
Conference, University of Manchester, 2-5 September 2009
Contact Paul Howard-Jones: Graduate School of Education, 35 Berkely Square, BS8
1JA, United Kingdom (e-mail: [email protected]).

Page 2
Neuroscience Literacy   2 
Abstract
Background: There is concern about the prevalence of neuromyths in education, but
little is known about how teachers think about the brain and how this may influence
their practice.
Aim: To further understanding of how teachers, at entry to the profession, think about
brain development and function.
Sample: 158 graduate trainee teachers at the end of their one-year course.
Methods: Preliminary semi-structured interviews contributed to the development of a
suitable survey instrument. Participants were then surveyed during one of their final
lectures.
Results: Trainees’ ideas reflected misconceptions in public circulation and notions
promoted by popular brain-based educational programmes. Most of the trainee
teachers in our survey did not accept, or were unsure, about whether mental activity
derives from biological brain function. Trainee teachers place equal importance on
home environment and education as determinants of educational outcome, with
genetics a significant but smaller influence than either. A follow up survey with a new
cohort of trainees confirmed that constructs about development are linked to a sense
of agency, with beliefs in strong genetic influence associated with stronger notions of
biologically-defined limits on pupil achievement.
Conclusions: In the absence of formal training, trainee teachers acquire their own
ideas about brain function, many of which are potentially detrimental to their practice
as teachers.

Page 3
Neuroscience Literacy   3 
Introduction
In 2002, the OECD’s Brain and Learning project drew international attention to the 
many myths and misconceptions that had arisen around the mind and brain outside of
the scientific community, including in schools. They defined the term “neuromyth” as 
being a “misconception generated by a misunderstanding, a misreading or a 
misquoting of facts scientifically established ....” (OECD, 2002, p111). Between 2005
and 2006, the ESRC-TLRP seminar series “Collaborative Frameworks in 
Neuroscience and Education” brought together over 400 teachers, neuroscientists, 
psychologists and policy-makers to discuss the potential for collaborative work that
might lead to improved educational and neuroscientific understanding. The
commentary arising from this seminar series proposed that education may have much
to gain from greater cognisance of the workings of the brain and improved dialogue
with those working in the neuroscience community (Howard-Jones, 2007). Amongst
other things, it was proposed this dialogue would help scrutinise neuro-myths and
evaluate programmes of “brain-based” learning. In May 2007, concerns about the
prevalence of classroom neuromythology were voiced again at a meeting an all-party
parliamentary group on scientific research in learning and education. Here, questions
were raised about whether initial teacher training should included a greater emphasis
on learning in terms of neural processes, as a way of deterring teachers from
unscientific and unhelpful brain-based notions. In response to concerns focusing on
Visual, Auditory and Kinaesthetic (VAK) learning styles, Alison Atkinson of the
Teacher Development Agency admitted “...we do not specify that neuroscience or 
VAK should be part of initial teacher training, but we do specify that teachers should
be up to date with knowledge on teaching and that they should engage with
educational research, the idea being that standards are thereby to some degree future-

Page 4
Neuroscience Literacy   4 
proofed....I do not think we see ourselves as including brain science in any format
within the standards under that very name, but we hope that teachers engage
constantly with what is happening in educational research” (Institute for the Future of
the Mind, 2007, p31). Such a “hands off” approach based on encouraging engagement 
with educational research may not, however, have adequately protected teachers and
their pupils from a host of inappropriate practices associated with unscientific ideas
about the brain. Indeed, it is possible to find examples of unhelpful brain-based ideas
being promoted rather than scrutinised in the educational research literature e.g.
(Moore & Hibbert, 2005).
It seems unlikely that this lack of neuroscientific training results in teachers
possessing no working ideas about brain function. Indeed, our informal “theory of 
brain” develops early, such that by the age of 4 we consider it as an internal body part 
involved with a range of distinctly mental acts, but do not differentiate between mind
and brain. During the school years, the concepts become increasingly differentiated
such that, by the age of 10-11 years old, children often consider there is some
cognitive function of the brain behind sensory-motor acts. Realisation grows with the
approach adulthood that the brain is essential for all behaviour, including
noncognitive involuntary responses such as fear and laughter (Johnson & Wellman,
1982). Experimental evidence demonstrates that adults possess an attraction to
explanations involving the brain, helping to explain the high profile of neuroscience in
the media and popular press (Weisberg, Keil, Goodstein, Rawson, & Gray, 2008).
Indeed, it has been suggested that this public interest is supporting the emergence of a
modern folk neuropsychology, a network of culturally shared concepts that people use
to explain their own, or another’s, mind and behaviour (Rodriguez, 2006). Such
explanations are expressed in ordinary everyday language. In metaphorical usage, the

Page 5
Neuroscience Literacy   5 
brain can be used in similar ways as the mind, e.g. as a container, a machine, a
memory recording medium or muscle. However, differences in the way mind and
brain are commonly used suggests a popular mind-brain dissociation and, thus, the
possibility of a complex mind-brain interrelationship. The brain is often portrayed as
the cause of a state of being (e.g. “now I’m brain dead” as an explanation for a mental 
state in which mistakes are being made). The fact that people often differentiate
between their experiencing self (Subject) and their body (self) is well established. To
some extent, phrases such as “this menu is confusing my brain” demonstrate how a 
reference to the brain can be used to support this subject-self dissociation, but the
story may be complicated than this. For example, in matters such as perception it is
still usually the Subject that sees, hears, feels etc., and yet references to the brain are
now being used to avoid the subject’s role here (e.g. “my brain can’t see it”) and, 
unlike the role of the body self and more like the role of the Subject, it can take on
direct agency (“My brain made me do it”). The use of the term brain is, of course, 
likely to be rapidly evolving, echoing the rate at which neuroscientific understanding
and public interest in neuroscience is burgeoning. For example, at the moment, it is
difficult to find examples of common speech that uses “brain” as an agent of either 
desires or intentions, although it may only be a matter of time before a popular
understanding of the reward system gives rise to phrases such as “my brain wants 
chocolate”. Such a cognitive semantic analysis suggests the existence of a potentially
complex, if not wholly scientific, interrelationship between matters of brain, mind and
behaviour amongst the adult public. Further evidence for this was provided by a
survey of the citizens of Rio de Janeiro carried out in 2002. Here, members of the
public and a sample of neuroscientists were asked to respond with agree, disagree or
don’t know to 95 assertions about the brain (Hurculano-Houzel, 2002). The survey

Page 6
Neuroscience Literacy   6 
revealed that the public, unlike the scientists, were evenly divided about the
usefulness of studying the brain to understand the mind, suggesting a range of
opinions regarding the brain-mind relationship. This was despite the same sample of
the public holding many concepts about the brain that concurred with scientific
opinion, such as the dependency of learning on attention, the association of different
brain regions with different cognitive functions and the non-stop operation of the
brain throughout the day and night. However, they were less sure of other basic
scientific concepts, such as the dependency of learning upon the modification of
connections between neurons. Assertions they generally agreed with, in opposition to
experts, also included the idea that emotions always disrupt reasoning (they are often
necessary for it), the existence of single memory system in the brain (there are several
systems), that hormones do not influence personality (they do) and that the brain
operates like a computer (when, in fact, memory and processing is distributed
throughout the brain, unlike a computer).
As well as being influenced by the prevailing folk neuropsychology and opinions in
general public domains, one can expect educators to have contact with an additional
range of information sources associated specifically with their profession, these may
also influence their constructions of mind and brain. Chief amongst these professional
influences are the “brain-based” educational products and programmes that have been
successfully marketed within schools in the last two decades, most of which appear to
have little scientific merit. Two of those attracting particular concern have already
been mentioned above: Visual, auditory and kinaesthetic (VAK) learning styles,
learning preferences based on left-brain/right-brain categorisation (Institute for the
Future of the Mind, 2007) and also educational kinaesthetics or ‘brain gym’ (Hyatt, 
2007). The latter, in addition to promoting concepts about “repatterning” the brain to

Page 7
Neuroscience Literacy   7 
promote literacy, also claims a strong relationship between the drinking of water and
learning. Other concerns include nutritional issues such as benefits of Omega 3 and
the effects of sugar (Howard-Jones, 2008). Another popular neuromyth is the
neuroscientific justification for stimulus-rich environments during the early years,
partly based on unscientific notions of critical periods for formal learning (Blakemore
& Frith, 2005).
Thus, there are clear reasons to assume teachers, and the trainee teachers who have
worked with them, possess concepts about the mind and brain despite this area being
absent from formal requirements for Initial Teacher Training. Moreover, some of
these concepts may be exclusively associated with their professional activities, thus
providing teachers with a potentially distinct set of notions about the brain that differs
from those of experts and/or the general public. Even leaving aside the common
implementation of teaching strategies claiming to be brain-based, notions of mind and
brain may be linked with educational attitudes and practices and are, therefore, a
valuable focus of investigation. For example, it has been pointed out that public
debate around dyslexia easily becomes polarised in terms of causes being either
biologically determined or not, and that the arguments become closely bound up with
whether dyslexia is amenable to educational remediation (Nicolson, 2005). It seems
possible, therefore, that strong beliefs in genetic predisposition and biologically
determined brain development are linked to teachers’ perceptions of the extent to 
which they can influence a learner’s progress. It has been reported that teachers 
believe genetics is a very important factor influencing their pupils’ development. A
survey received replies from 667 UK teachers asked to what extent nature (genes) or
nurture (environment) was responsible for various pupil outcomes (Walker & Plomin,
2005). The percentage of teachers who perceived that genetics accounts for at least

Page 8
Neuroscience Literacy   8 
half of the influence, was 87% for personality, 94% for intelligence and the same
figure for learning difficulties, 43% for behaviour problems and 91% for mental
illness. Only 1-9% saw these behaviours as due to “all genes” and 0-1% as “all 
environment”. Walker and Plomin used their results to suggest that most teachers,
despite a lack of formal genetics in their training and the frequent use of misleading
phrases in the media (e.g. “dyslexia gene”), possess a balanced view of the 
importance of both genetics and environment as influences upon outcome. The survey
did not ask what remaining proportion of environmental influence derives from
formal education, although the authors of the study claim that teachers are undaunted
by their perceptions of genetic influence. Indeed, contrary to such concerns, Walker
and Plomin suggest that teachers want to know more about such biological influences,
believing such knowledge can support their teaching, with 82% of teachers in their
survey claiming they would change their method of tracking and instructing a child if
they knew he/she suffered from a genetically influenced learning difficulty.
In a world where everyday language promotes contradictory ideas about the mind-
brain relationship, and even professional development cannot be relied upon to deliver
valid neuroscientific concepts, it can be expected that teachers’ ideas about the brain
diverge from conventional scientific thinking. So how do teachers talk and think about
the brain?
Preliminary interviews
This initial investigation began with some informal semi-structured interviews with a
primary school headteacher, 6 teachers (3 primary, 3 secondary) each with several
years experience and 6 trainee secondary teachers about to embark on their career.
These were informal conversational affairs. We started by asking them what sort of

Page 9
Neuroscience Literacy   9 
ability range they had encountered as teachers, and what they thought produced such a
diverse range of achievement. Some of our group, as might be predicted from the
survey by Walker and Plomin, were convinced that genetics should be considered as
the key factor or, as one primary school teacher put it:
“I believe it’s about genes, I believe you do see intelligent children from 
intelligent parents”
Our headteacher also put forward genetics as the key reason for such diversity, but
suggested home environment was the next big factor:
“It is genetically inherited. If I try to do calculus, high math, with some 
children it wouldn’t matter how much I try they wouldn’t understand it and 
then their motivation would decrease….I think it’s genetics…genetics is a 
very high one and motivation and the environment at home is another one”
Indeed most of our group considered there was a balanced influence between genetics
and environment on brain development, with a secondary school teacher explaining:
“…. you’ve got the combination of nature and nurture because…it’s a horrible 
kind of a sweeping statement and generalization…but often more educated 
parents would have more resources to help their children when they are
young..”
It was also clear, however, that when different teachers discussed “environment”, they 
rarely used the term as scientists do, i.e. as an umbrella term that covers all influences
derived from social and physical settings. It could sometimes refer to just influences
derived from the home:

Page 10
Neuroscience Literacy   10 
Interviewer: “Why do you think there is this range between students in the 
classroom?”
Primary school teacher: “I think it has a lot to do with the environment”
Interviewer: “Environment – what exactly?”
Primary School teacher: “Kind of their home environment. Perhaps the way 
they have been brought up and the area that they live in, perhaps, but then
also, I think, it could be genetic as well in their families. So it could be to do
with the make up of the family”
Or, when used in the educational sense, the term “environment” could mean the 
influence or stimulus provided by the teacher and classroom, as in this comment by a
trainee teacher:
“I think maybe in a very enriched environment probably the brain will be
bigger and heavier (from) lots of different kinds of stimuli, lots of changing
activities very rapidly, a really good mixture of conventional grammar and
spelling, at the same time doing fun stuff like music, images, sound.”
Ideas around the plasticity of the brain were diverse and sometimes complex, with the
same trainee expressing how mental ability can develop, but uneasy about how such
changes might be reflected at biological level:
“I think the brain must be able to change to be honest. You see people who 
don’t have hands so they (learn to) use their toes to be able to paint. So you
work about the problem, not getting rid of it but you are certainly working
about it. ….It’s like a car and you have the engine and you can supe it up and
rewire it and polish it but it is the same car” 

Page 11
Neuroscience Literacy   11 
Concepts about plasticity were also frequently linked to age, as in this comment by a
secondary teacher:
“I think your brain is always developing and I think it can develop in reaction 
to your environment so I think it can cause your brain to sort of develop more
maybe when you are young…
Ageing of the brain was mentioned by some as providing a limitation on learning
arising from lack of brain plasticity although, again, our teachers were unclear about
how this worked. Explanations often touched upon neuromythology:
Interviewer: Do you know how the brain grows?
Secondary school teacher: I know that after you’re 18, as far as I know, you 
don’t regenerate brain cells anymore, so people shouldn’t hit you on the head.
Interviewer: If you’re not hit on the head, can you learn new skills? For
example, if brain cells are not regenerating, would that hinder your learning
and your skills?
Secondary school teacher: I don’t think so, no, I mean we only use 10% of our 
brain anyway apparently, that’s what I’ve been taught.
Interviewer: Why don’t we use the other 90%?
Secondary school teacher: I have no idea.
The extent to which brain development was open to educational intervention proved
an interesting area for discussion. On the one hand, as demonstrated above, direct
questions about whether an improvement in ability might correspond to physical brain
changes generally met with agreement. On the other hand, when learning difficulties

Page 12
Neuroscience Literacy   12 
were associated with differences in brain function, teachers appeared unclear about
the effectiveness of educational solutions and thought more in terms of “coping”, as in 
this exchange with another secondary school teacher:
Interviewer: Does knowing this (a learning difficulty) has something to do
with the brain change the way you’re looking at the student?
Secondary school teacher: “It certainly I suppose changes how you deal with 
it because, if you were told that it’s entirely to do with the brain then you’re 
kind of looking at ways to cope with them that would make things easier for
them in the classroom.”
There appeared to be a type of “all or none” theorising about problems being either
biological in nature or not:
Interviewer: What are the options in front of you do you think?
Secondary school trainee teacher: I think…I suppose yes…if there’s a 
biological problem, things like diet, drugs. I don’t really like the idea of drugs, 
but I think some people do see them as a readily good option for some
children.
Interviewer: What exactly do you mean by diet?
Secondary school trainee teacher: I mean avoiding colourants, avoiding fizzy
drinks, avoiding too much sugar. So, you know, trying also I suppose fish oils
as well, I am real believer in all of that.

Page 13
Neuroscience Literacy   13 
One teacher explicitly categorised causal factors of learning difficulties involving the
brain as medical, possibly implying they lay beyond the capability of her professional
domain:
“some of the causes are medical and have to do with the brain and some 
disorders are due to the environment….”. 
This medicalisation of learning difficulties occurred in other interviews, linked to a
reduced sense of agency and the use of medical words such as “cure”, “symptoms”, 
“diagnosis”:
Interviewer: “What do you think causes pupils to have special needs, other 
than the environment?”
Secondary school trainee teacher: “It is a diagnosable condition so it must be 
very physiological, neurological as well and related to the brain…I don’t know 
if you can cure kids with symptoms, by giving them strategies to get around it.
If teachers have dyslexic students they may cope with it but you cannot cure
it.”
Our headteacher explained that a sense of reduced agency might be why explanations
involving the brain, which he perceived as deterministic, were less popular with some
educators. He suggested that factors perceived to be less amenable to their influence
might feature less in teachers’ discussions of cause (note that this teacher, like several 
others we spoke to, had previously used the word ‘environment’ to refer to home 
environment):
“As a teacher you are faced with classes of children and you do your very best,
I think teachers have an understanding that the environment and the emotional

Page 14
Neuroscience Literacy   14 
responses of children have to do with things outside their control but they have
to try to work it out. It’s not much use if you are faced with a child and say it
has to do with a dysfunction of the brain because if you are a class teacher that
wouldn’t help.”
Teachers sometimes associated the amelioration of brain-referenced learning
difficulties with controlling diet and this may be due to a belief, expressed by one
teacher, that ‘fixable’ issues to do with the brain revolved around chemical imbalance: 
Interviewer: When we were talking about dyslexia, we were saying that the
working memory of the brain might not be functioning well. If we know the
reasons, can we fix it?
Secondary school teacher: I guess it would depend if it’s a chemical 
imbalance.
Interviewer: What do you mean by a chemical imbalance?
Secondary school teacher: Well…if what’s there in the brain is more of a
certain chemical or less than a certain chemical then yes you can fix it. But if
it’s a structure in the brain then I would imagine you can’t fix it.
When asked what foods were good for the brain, the issue of fish oils came up
regularly, but also some more surprising ones:
Interviewer: Can you improve this connectivity in the brain?
Primary school teacher: Probably there is some food.
Interviewer: Like what?

Page 15
Neuroscience Literacy   15 
Primary school teacher: There are lots of traditional recipes to neuralize the
brain.
Interviewer: Like what?
Primary school teacher: Like walnut…it has the shape of the brain…and also 
I think there are some components of walnut that kind of help to improve the
neurons in your brain or to kind of…moisturize it1.
More predictably, our teachers also referred to the effects of too much sugar and not
enough water on children, as in these comments by two trainee secondary school
teachers:
“When they have too much sugar in breaks, they come to class very active. If 
they are dehydrated – dehydration is a serious issue”
“Sugary food ….after break time they are a bit lively” 
Survey of trainee teachers’ neuroscience literacy
Our initial interviews had revealed a mixture of ideas strongly influenced by those in
public and educational domains, and also raised questions about how constructions
around brain function and development might influence teachers’ sense of their own 
agency. To understand more, a survey was undertaken to explore the level of
knowledge and understanding about the brain amongst a sample of trainee teachers
about to qualify and begin their careers in UK secondary schools.
Method
                                                          
1 Apart from its visual resemblance to the wrinkled exterior of the human cortex, this idea may be
related to the fact the walnut has a higher Omega 3 content than any common nut (Davis & Kris-
Etherington, 2003)

Page 16
Neuroscience Literacy   16 
Participants were 158 trainee teachers (102 females, 52 males, 4 unspecified)
attending an Educational Studies lecture on a PGCE course at an established centre of
teacher training in England. Trainees were asked to complete a survey consisting of
38 assertions (15 correct, 16 incorrect, 7 open to subjective opinion) to which
participants were asked to respond agree, don’t know or disagree. 16 assertions were
adapted from a study of the neuroscience literacy of the South American public
(Hurculano-Houzel, 2002). This allowed comparison of trainee responses to those of
the subgroup of this South American sample that were also educated to graduate level.
Adaptation of these assertions involved straightforward improvements in expression
to support clarification (the published assertions had been translated from Spanish)
and occasional reversal of sense to allow balancing of correct and incorrect assertions.
Additionally, in two instances, examples were provided to help clarify meaning.
Assertions were chosen from Hurculano-Houzel et al, on the basis that, in the broadest
sense, they focused on learning (e.g. those dealing with memory) or aspects of
behaviour that could be associated with behaviour management (e.g. those dealing
with personality and emotion). These were combined with 15 additional assertions
representing ideas promoted by popular brain-based educational programmes,
prevalent neuromyths and other concepts identified in preliminary interviews and
previous research (e.g. Pickering & Howard-Jones, 2007).
Additionally, the 6 assertions of subjective opinion from Hurculano-Houzel et al.
about the mind-brain relationship were included, together with an extra one. This
extra assertion arose from concerns about biological determinism similar to those
discussed above, but this time in relation to learner agency. It explored trainees’ 

Page 17
Neuroscience Literacy   17 
opinions about whether individuals should be held responsible for behaviours
associated with a developmental difference in their brain function.
Presentation order of assertions in the survey was randomised and correctness of
assertions was balanced. Respondents were asked to select either “yes”, “no” or 
“don’t know” as the answer that most closely reflected their opinion. At the end of the
survey, they were also asked to indicate their specialist subject, gender, whether they
read popular science magazines and/or newspapers, and how many books they read
per month (0, ½, 1, 2, 3, 4 or more). Finally, they were asked to estimate the
percentage contribution of home environment, school environment, genes, and ‘other’ 
influences upon educational outcome, and which (if any) of three major brain-based
educational approaches they had encountered in their schools (Multiple Intelligences,
Learning Styles (e.g. VAK), Brain Gym).
Trainee teachers attending an Educational Studies lecture in the final weeks of their
PGCE training course were asked to complete the survey prior to the commencement
of the lecture. 158 student teachers attended the lecture and completed the survey.
Results
Respondents (N = 158, 102 females, 52 males, 4 unspecified) had been trained to
teach pupils (aged 11-18) in the specialist subjects of science, maths, modern foreign
languages, English, history, geography, citizenship, music and religious education
(N=32,22,22,17,16,15,13,10,6 respectively) with 5 respondents failing to clearly
report their specialism. The percentage of respondents who had encountered concepts
of Multiple Intelligences, Learning Style and Brain Gym in schools were 56%, 83%
and 58% respectively.

Page 18
Neuroscience Literacy   18 
Responses to assertions of subjective opinion about the mind-brain relationship, and
to our assertion regarding learner agency are shown in Table 1, together with data
provided by Hurculano-Houzel et al. for their sample of the public who had been
educated at graduate level.
TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE
The mean number of correct responses of trainee teachers to the 16 general assertions
about the brain selected from the survey by Hurculano-Houzel et al. was 9.15
(SD=2.85). The percentage of trainee teachers responding agree, don’t know and 
disagree to this selection of general assertions about the brain are shown in Table 2.
Again, for comparison, these are shown with the results for graduates from the
Hurculano-Houzel et al. survey of the public.
TABLE 2 ABOUT HERE
The mean number of correct responses of trainee teachers to the 15 assertions about
the brain relating to common neuromyths and misunderstanding relevant to education
was 5.13 (SD=2.15). The percentage of trainee teachers responding agree, don’t know 
and disagree to these assertions are shown in Table 3.
TABLE 3 ABOUT HERE
The mean number (with standard deviations in parentheses) of correct responses of
trainee teachers who were newspaper readers (N=109) to the 16 general assertions and
15 neuromythological assertions about the brain were 9.19 (3.02) and 5.09 (2.13)
respectively, compared with 9.04 (2.47) and 5.21 (2.21) for trainees who did not read
newspapers (N=49). T-tests showed the differences in mean scores for these two
groups were not significant. Similarly, the mean number (with standard deviations in

Page 19
Neuroscience Literacy   19 
parentheses) of correct responses of trainee teachers who were science magazine
readers (N=26) to the 16 general assertions and 15 neuromythological assertions
about the brain were 9.85 (3.38) and 5.69 (2.05) respectively, compared with 9.01
(2.73) and 5.02 (2.16) for those who did not read science magazines (N=132). T-tests
showed the differences in mean scores for these two groups were not significant. No
statistically significant association could be found between numbers of books read and
numbers of correct responses to either the 16 general assertions or the 15
neuromythological assertions about the brain.
A scatter plot and Spearman’s rank correlation analysis was undertaken to test for
association between individuals’ scores for the 16 general assertions about the brain 
and the 15 assertions about the brain relating to common neuromyths and
misunderstanding relevant to education. This revealed a statistically significant
correlation between these two measures of participants knowledge (Spearman’s rho = 
0.43, p<0.0001).
The mean percentage contribution (standard deviations in parentheses) to educational
outcomes that trainee teachers attributed to education, genes and home environment
were 36.9(16.7), 25.5 (14.9) and 36.4 (15.5). This data is displayed in Fig. 1. 14
trainees responded in the “other” category, but all naming influences that might be 
placed under home environment (e.g. “social status”, ”community”), or both 
education and home environment (“experience”).
FIG 1 ABOUT HERE
Discussion

Page 20
Neuroscience Literacy   20 
Opinions regarding the six mind-brain assertions revealed considerable uncertainty.
Around three-quarters did not consider that consciousness was possible without a
brain, and only 15% wished to consider the mind as arising from the action of a spirit
or soul on the brain. However, most did not agree that “state of mind” reflects brain 
state, that the mind is in this way, or any other, a product of brain function, or that the
mind can be studied through studying brain activity. This conflicts markedly with
current scientific opinion, including the opinions of the neuroscientists sampled by
Hurculano-Houzel et al. (2002) but also, as can be seen in Table 1, the majority
opinion of the South American public they sampled who had benefited from graduate
level education. It should be noted that these issues are matters of opinion, rather than
scientific fact and the results from Hurculano-Houzel et al.’s survey showed no effect 
of education level on respondents’ confidence in a meaningful brain-mind
relationship. Here, it is tentatively suggested that many of these trainee teachers may
have been recently impressed by the social complexity of behaviour in the classroom.
This may have caused them to be less certain than other non-specialists about a model
of mental activity based on biological function, which might seem an overly-reductive
approach to explaining cognition and behaviour in educational contexts. However,
this is a purely speculative explanation. Uncertainties regarding the brain-behaviour
relationship were also reflected in the large number of trainees who were undecided
about whether students should be considered responsible for behaviours associated
with a developmental disorder (55%).
Trainees’ views on the 16 general assertions about the brain were characterised
similarly to the sample of the graduate public reported on by Hurculano-Houzel et al.
(2002), with a few exceptions. More members of our sample correctly disagreed with
“Keeping a phone number in memory until dialling, recalling recent events & distant

Page 21
Neuroscience Literacy   21 
experiences, all use the same memory system”. However, this may have been due to
better comprehension of the question, due to the authors providing examples in the
survey given to trainee teachers. A high number in our sample also agreed with the
statement “Learning is not due to the addition of new cells to the brain”. However, the 
incorrectness of this statement has recently become more moot, making subjects’ 
responses to it potentially difficult to interpret in terms of general neuroscientific
awareness. Conventionally, it has been assumed by scientists that changes in neural
connectivity are sufficient to explain learning but well-publicised research during the
present decade has also revealed examples of neurogenesis associated with memory
formation (Shors et al., 2001). However, most trainee teachers in our sample either
disagreed or were undecided about the more conventional explanation based on neural
connectivity, and this tends to suggest that the popularity of the neurogenesis
explanation arose out of a general lack of understanding, which surpasses the levels
found by Hurculano-Houzel et al. in the general population, rather than over-
interpretation or confusion arising from the latest scientific findings. Perhaps the most
surprising response of our trainee teachers to these general assertions was that most
did not agree (43%), or did not know (13%), whether it was necessary to pay attention
to something in order to learn it. In the sense of learning that is commonly used in
education, it is difficult to imagine how learning without attention can occur. This
may be due to an interpretation of ‘attention’ in a more educational sense (i.e. paying 
attention to the teacher), although the assertion said clearly ‘attention to it’. 
Alternatively, this response may indicate the rise of a new misunderstanding about the
brain related to implicit learning. Work with artificial grammars, in which participants
are able to acquire grammatical rules by observing examples of artificial language,
demonstrates our ability to learn implicitly, i.e. without being able to report explicitly

Page 22
Neuroscience Literacy   22 
what we have learnt (Johnstone & Shanks, 2001). Such experiments have contributed
to enthusiastic calls for more educational focus on implicit learning (e.g. Claxton,
1998). However, there are considerable barriers to the practical application of such
ideas, making their usefulness to education questionable and causing some scientific
authorities to label them a new source of neuromyth (Goswami, 2004). A non-
specialist interpretation of the phenomenon of implicit learning might involve ideas
about absorbing information and concepts from the environment without attending to
them, but such ideas have no scientific basis. For example, in the artificial grammar
scenario, formal rules may be acquired without the learner consciously formulating
them, but the learner must pay considerable attention to the examples of artificial
language in order to facilitate this. In a more ‘real world’ context, we may also
implicitly develop understanding about, for example, the motivations of people
around us, without being able to articulate how we have achieved this. Again,
however, this is only possible by paying attention to their behaviour. “Implicit
learning” does not equate to “learning without attention”. Even given the popular rise 
of ideas about implicit learning, it seemed somewhat surprising to the authors that
43% of our sample of trainee teachers, towards the end of their training, appeared to
consider that their pupils might learn without paying due attention, and this finding
may justify further research.
In some instances, trainees’ opinions about the 15 assertions about the brain relating
to common neuromyths showed a majority in agreement with present scientific
opinion. For example, 62% considered that “Extended rehearsal of some mental
processes can change the shape and structure of some parts of the brain”, a fact which 
has been demonstrated in at least two well-reported instances (Draganski et al., 2004;
Maguire et al., 2000). Additionally, 63% considered (correctly) that the production of

Page 23
Neuroscience Literacy   23 
new connections in the brain can continue into old age, fact which can be assumed on
the basis that learning relies on synaptic plasticity, and learning can be shown to
continue throughout life. There was also a majority (55%) able to agree with the
current notion amongst neuroscientists that sensitive, rather than critical, periods exist
for learning, such that there is no clearly defined window of opportunity for learning
outside which progress is impossible, just periods when learning can be more
efficiently achieved. However, it is also worth noting that the contexts of learning for
which even sensitive periods have been observed are chiefly those involving primary
sensory or motor function, rather than the higher types of learning process that are
usually the subject of formal education (for further discussion, see Blakemore & Frith,
2005, p26-36).
Most trainees had, however, already come into contact with approaches such as
multiple intelligences, learning styles and “Brain Gym“ that involve concepts 
claiming a brain-basis, and this may explain the large numbers of trainees suffering
misconceptions in related areas. This contact had occurred by the end of a one-year
course, presumably through school placements. This speaks of the extent to which
learning styles, “Brain Gym“ and multiple intelligences have become prevalent in
state schools in the UK, despite their dubious scientific basis (see Geake, 2008; Hyatt,
2007; Waterhouse, 2006 respectively for a critical review of these ideas). Such
contact may explain why 82% of trainees considered that “Individuals learn better
when they receive information in their preferred learning style”, even though an 
extensive review of the educational evidence is unable to support the educational
value of identifying learning styles (Coffield, Moseley, Gall, & Ecclestone, 2004).
Moreover, a recent psychological investigation of the VAK principle tested recall of
information presented in the three different styles (Kratzig & Arbuthnott, 2006) and

Page 24
Neuroscience Literacy   24 
showed no benefit of having material presented in one’s preferred learning style. It
may be, as agreed with by 79% of trainees, that individuals show preferences for the
mode in which they receive information but, as concluded by this scientific study,
identifying these preferences serves no demonstrable educational purpose and
attempts to focus on learning styles appear to be “wasted effort”. Most trainees (60%)
also revealed their belief in the usefulness of hemispheric dominance (left brain, right
brain) as a means to explain individual differences amongst learners. This belief is
also used as a learning style approach to categorizing learners and as a means to
differentiate teaching strategies accordingly. It is true that some tasks can be
associated with extra activity that is predominantly in one hemisphere or the other
(e.g. language can be considered in most individuals to be left lateralised). However,
no part of the brain is ever normally inactive in the sense that no blood flow is
occurring. Furthermore, performance in most everyday tasks, including learning tasks,
require both hemispheres to work together in a sophisticated parallel fashion. The
division of people into left-brained and right-brained takes the misunderstanding one
stage further and there is no reliable evidence that categorisation based on
hemispheric dominance is helpful for teaching and learning.
Although most trainees (63%) were correct in believing that vigorous exercise can
improve mental function, there was also a majority in favour of the concept that co-
ordination exercises can help integrate the functions of left and right hemisphere. This
latter assertion cannot be supported by reviews of the scientific literature (Arter &
Jenkins, 1979; Bochner, 1978; Cohen, 1969; Hammill, Goodman, & Wiederholt,
1974; Kavale & Forness, 1987; Sullivan, 1972), yet over a third of trainees (35%) felt
this type of exercise could contribute to development of literacy skills, with most
(56%) expressing uncertainty as to whether this might be possible or not. This belief

Page 25
Neuroscience Literacy   25 
contributes to the dubious theoretical approach of programmes such as “Brain Gym”, 
but is not supported by scientific evidence (Hyatt, 2007). Approaches such as “Brain 
Gym” also promote the drinking of water as way to support learning. Apart from 
circumstances involving vigorous exercise, ill health or unusually hot weather, there is
no evidence of children suffering from voluntary dehydration in the classroom or the
cognitive effects associated with it. However, the prevalence of myths around the
health-giving properties of water (Valtin, 2002) and those that now associate it with
learning, may help explain why 39% of trainees were not sure if their brain would
shrink if they drank less than 6-8 glasses a day, with a further 18 % agreeing that it
would. In other nutritional areas of interest to educators, most trainees were unaware
(22%) or unsure (45%) about the fact that habitual caffeine use suppresses cognition
rather enhances it. In fact, children commonly experience caffeine withdrawal (James,
1997). Heatherley and colleagues showed that children aged 9-10 who habitually
consumed the equivalent of no more than 2 cans a day of cola demonstrated decreased
alertness relative to low users (Heatherley, Hancock, & Rogers, 2006). Their alertness
only rose to baseline levels when they had received some caffeine and then, of course,
only temporarily. In terms of the potentially positive effects of Omega 3 supplements,
there is currently no scientifically valid evidence showing positive outcomes in the
general population, with studies involving children with developmental disorders
revealing mixed results. Nonetheless, it appears 23% of our trainees already believe
this is the case, with the majority (54%) unclear about this issue. Most trainees (63%)
believed in the myth that children are less attentive after sugary drinks and snacks. On
the contrary, although certain food additives have been shown to increase
hyperactivity amongst children (McCann et al., 2007), sugary drinks and snacks are

Page 26
Neuroscience Literacy   26 
associated with increases in children’s ability to attend (Busch, Taylor, Kanarek, &
Holcomb, 2002).
Perhaps, however, the most worrying result of our survey is that (only) just less than
half of the trainees (49%) disagreed with the statement “Learning problems associated 
with developmental differences in brain function cannot be remediated by education”, 
with 41% undecided and 9% of the opinion that this was true. This suggests that a
teacher’s knowledge of a pupil’s developmental differences may often diminish their 
belief in the potential for positive change, as if some biological barrier has been
exposed. Current perspectives in developmental cognitive neuroscience avoid
predictive mechanisms of biological cause and effect, emphasise the complexity of
interrelation between biological systems and environments such as those provided by
education, and highlight the enduring possibility of mitigation. Further insights into
respondents belief in biologically determined outcomes arises from their estimates of
the percentage contribution to educational outcomes that could be attributed to
education, genes and home environment. Our sample of trainee teachers considered,
on average, that only 25% of educational outcome was due to genetic issues. This
contrasts considerably with the findings of Walker and Plomin (2005) who concluded
that “teachers view nature to be at least as important as nurture”. In their survey, 
Walker and Plomin presented 667 primary school teachers with a list of five broad
categories of behavioural traits: personality, intelligence, behaviour problems,
learning difficulties, and mental illness. Respondents were asked about the extent to
which each trait was influenced by genes (nature) or the environment (nurture). The
percentage of teachers who reported genetics were at least as important as
environment were 87%, 94%, 43%, 94%, and 91%, respectively. However, we
suggest that Walker and Plomin’s participants may have been confused by what was 

Page 27
Neuroscience Literacy   27 
meant by the term “environment”, which can have a range of disparate meanings in 
education, most of which are narrower than its meaning within the field of genetics,
and many of which may not even include the teacher’s efforts. The difference
between our results and those of Walker and Plomin may illustrate an important
challenge likely to be involved with most interdisciplinary studies at the interface of
neuroscience/genetics and education. In such investigations, the same language and
terms may be used in diverse ways by the different disciplines involved, influencing
the data collected and its interpretation in unexpected ways. Walker and Plomin
suggest that their “finding that teachers view nature to be at least as important as 
nurture does not imply teachers, whose job is to educate children and nurture their
potential, believe that their efforts have no impact – or they would not be in the field
of education.” In supporting their argument, that a belief in genetics as the key
determinant of outcome does not reduce teachers’ sense of agency, they point to
teachers’ individual comments about how they are making their best teaching efforts
irrespective of their nature/nurture perspective, and to teachers’ requests for more
information to help facilitate earlier intervention. In order to understand this issue
further, we looked more carefully at the responses of the small minority in our study
(N=12, or 8% ) that did believe genetics was at least or more important than home
environment and education put together, and in particular how they responded to our
question regarding biological determinism. For this small sample, equal numbers
(N=4, or 33%) agreed, disagreed and were undecided about whether learning
problems associated with developmental differences in brain function cannot be
remediated by education. This was in contrast to the rest of the group, of whom only
6% agreed, 40% were undecided, and 51% disagreed. In this light, Walker and
Plomin’s other finding, that 82% percent of teachers considered knowledge that a 

Page 28
Neuroscience Literacy   28 
pupil had a genetically influenced learning difficulty would cause them to change
their teaching strategies, takes on a potentially more ominous significance. An
alternative interpretation might be that, if such knowledge reduces a teacher’s sense of 
agency, the changes in teaching strategy they refer to may reflect diminished
expectations of academic progress.
There was a clear association between scores for general knowledge about the brain
and for a correct understanding of concepts associated with common classroom
neuromyths. This suggests that having a basic knowledge of brain function may
provide some protection against the most prevalent of misconceptions currently
influencing educational thinking and classroom practice.
Follow up survey on genetic beliefs and biological limits to achievement
A smaller second survey was devised to further investigate whether genetic beliefs
were related to a reduced sense of teachers’ agency. This survey was carried out in 
2009 with a new group of trainee secondary school teachers approximately halfway
through their training (N=166, 103 females, 58 males, 5 unspecified). As before, these
trainee teachers were asked to estimate the mean % contribution to educational
outcomes that they would attribute to education, genes, home environment and
“other”.  It then asked them to rate their agreement with 2 statements on 5-point
Likert scale:
There is a biological limit to what some individuals can achieve in their education
There is no biological limit to what any individual can achieve in their education
Mean percentage attributions of educational achievement to education, genes, home
environment and “other” were (with standard deviations in parentheses) were 36.8

Page 29
Neuroscience Literacy   29 
(12.1), 21.1 (10.9), 41.1 (13.4), 1.5 (5.1). This was a similar set of figures to those
obtained in the previous survey for this question, confirming that genetic influence is
viewed by trainee teachers as a lesser influence than either educational or home
environment. Responses in the “other” category were almost entirely related to the
influence of friends and peers, a social environmental factor that cannot be considered
as directly genetic.
Trainees’ responses to the two statements were scored for their belief in a biological 
limit to a learner’s achievement, i.e. level of agreement with the first (1 to 4 points)
and level of disagreement with the second (1 to 4 points). This produced a score out of
8 for each participant (mean = 4.41, SD = 1.86). Spearman rank correlation analysis
revealed a statistically significant association between the percentage of educational
outcome trainees attributed to genetic factors and their beliefs in a biological limit to
what a learner can achieve (Spearman’s rho = 0.22, p = 0.005).
Conclusions
This study has revealed that most respondents in a sample of trainees towards the end
of a 1-year PGCE course had already come into contact with brain-based ideas in their
short period of training, despite these forming no part of the formal college-based part
of the course. Moreover, a large number of trainees already possessed a range of
misunderstandings about the brain, many of which can be found in educational
resources, practices and programmes being successfully marketed within UK schools.
Higher levels of general knowledge about the brain were associated with increased
resistance to such ideas, suggesting that the inclusion of some basic neuroscience in
initial teacher training may help inoculate trainees against common educational
neuromyths and the poor practice associated with them. Contrary to findings of a

Page 30
Neuroscience Literacy   30 
study of primary school teachers, most trainees considered genetic issues to be less
important than either education or home environment as factors influencing
educational outcomes. The minority who placed great emphasis on the role of genetics
in educational outcome were characterised by a reduced confidence that learning
problems associated with developmental differences could benefit from educational
attempts at remediation. A follow up study confirmed that teachers’ constructs about 
development are linked to their sense of agency, with beliefs in strong genetic
influence associated with stronger notions of biologically-defined limits on pupil
achievement.
References
Arter, J. A., & Jenkins, J. R. (1979). Differential diagnosis - prescriptive teaching: A
critical appraisal. Review of Educational Research, 49, 517-555.
Blakemore, S. J., & Frith, U. (2005). The Learning Brain. Oxford: Blackwell.
Bochner, S. (1978). Ayres, sensory integration and learning disorders: A question of
theory and practice. Australian Journal of Mental Retardation, 5(2), 41-45.
Busch, C. R., Taylor, H. A., Kanarek, R. B., & Holcomb, P. J. (2002). The effects of a
confectionery snack on attention in young boys. Physiology & Behavior, 77(2-
3), PII S0031-9384(0002)00882-X.
Claxton, G. (1998). Knowing without knowing why. The Psychologist, 11(5), 217-
220.
Coffield, F., Moseley, D., Hall, E., & Ecclestone, K. (2004). Learning styles and
pedagogy in post-16 learning: A systematic and critical review (Report no.
041543). London: Learning and Skills Research Centre.
Cohen, S. A. (1969). Studies in visual perception and reading in disadvantaged
children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 2, 498-507.

Page 31
Neuroscience Literacy   31 
Davis, B. C., & Kris-Etherington, P. M. (2003). Achieving optimal essential fatty acid
status in vegetarians: Current knowledge and practical implications. The
American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 78(Supplement), 640S-646S.
Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., & May, A. (2004).
Changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature, 427, 311-312.
Geake, J. G. (2008). Neuromythologies in education. Educational Research, 50(2).
Goswami, U. (2004). Neuroscience and education. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 74, 1-14.
Hammill, D., Goodman, L., & Wiederholt, J. L. (1974). The Reading Teacher. 27,
469-478.
Heatherley, S. V., Hancock, K. M. F., & Rogers, P. J. (2006). Psychostimulant and
other effects of caffeine in 9- to 11-year-old children. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(2), 135-142.
Howard-Jones, P. A. (2007). Neuroscience and education: Issues and opportunities.
London: Teaching and Learning Research Programme.
Howard-Jones, P. A. (2008, 13th June). Brain Food. TES Magazine.
Hurculano-Houzel, S. (2002). Do you know your brain? A survey on Public
Neuroscience Literacy at the closing of the decade of the brain. The
Neuroscientist, 8(2), 98-110.
Hyatt, K. J. (2007). Brain Gym: Building stronger brains or wishful thinking?
Remedial and Special Education, 28(2), 117-124.
Institute_for_the_Future_of_the_Mind. (2007). Transcript of keynote seminar of the
all-party parliamentary group on scientific research in learning and education
"Brain-Science in the Classroom". Portcullis House, London: Institute for the
Future of the Mind.

Page 32
Neuroscience Literacy   32 
James, J. E. (1997). Understanding Caffeine: A biobehavioural analysis. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Johnson, C. N., & Wellman, H. M. (1982). Children's developing conceptions of the
mind and brain. Child Development, 53(1), 222-234.
Johnstone, T., & Shanks, D. R. (2001). Abstractionist and processing accounts of
implicit learning. Cognitive Psychology, 42, 61-112.
Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (1987). Substance over style: Assessing the efficacy
of modality testing and teaching. Exceptional Children, 54, 228-239.
Kratzig, G. P., & Arbuthnott, K. D. (2006). Perceptual learning style and learning
proficiency: A test of the hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology,
98(1), 238-246.
Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. S., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J.,
Frackowiak, R. S., et al. (2000). Navigation related structural change in the
hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
(USA), 97(8), 4398-4403.
McCann, D., Barrett, A., Cooper, A., Crumpler, D., Dalen, L., Grimshaw, K., et al.
(2007). Food additives and hyperactive behaviour in 3-year-old and 8/9-year-
old children in the community: a randomised, double-blinded, placebo-
controlled trial. [Article]. Lancet, 370(9598), 1560-1567.
Moore, H., & Hibbert, F. (2005). Mind Boggling! Considering the possibilities of
brain gym in learning to play an instrument. British Journal of Music
Education, 22(3), 249-267.
Nicolson, R. (2005). Dyslexia: Beyond the myth. The Psychologist, 18(11), 658-659.
OECD. (2002). Understanding the Brain:Towards a New Learning Science. Paris:
OECD Publications.

Page 33
Neuroscience Literacy   33 
Pickering, S. J., & Howard-Jones, P. (2007). Educators' views on the role of
neuroscience in education: Findings from a study of UK and international
perspectives. Mind, Brain and Education, 1(3), 109-113.
Rodriguez, P. (2006). Talking brains: a cognitive semantic analysis of an emerging
folk neuropsychology. Public Understanding of Science, 15(3), 301-330.
Shors, T. J., Miesegaes, G., Beylin, A., Zhao, M. R., Rydel, T., & Gould, E. (2001).
Neurogenesis in the adult is involved in the formation of trace memories
Nature, 414(6866), 938-938.
Sullivan, J. (1972). The effects of Kephart's perceptual motor-training on a reading
clinic sample. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 5(10), 32-38.
Valtin, H. (2002). "Drink at least eight glasses of water a day." Really? Is there
scientific evidence for "8x8"? American Journal of Regulatory Comparitive
Physiology, 283, 993-1004.
Walker, S. O., & Plomin, R. (2005). The Nature-Nurture Question: Teachers'
Perceptions of How Genes and the Environment Influence Educationally
Relevant Behaviour. Educational Psychology, 25(5), 509-516.
Waterhouse, L. (2006). Multiple intelligences, the Mozart effect, and emotional
intelligence: A critical review. Educational Psychologist, 41(4), 207-225.
Weisberg, D. S., Keil, F. C., Goodstein, J., Rawson, E., & Gray, J. (2008). The
Seductive Lure of Neuroscience Explanations. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 20(3), 470-477.
 
 

Page 34
Neuroscience Literacy   34 
Trainee teachers
Public with graduate education
(from Hurculano-Houzel, 2002)
agree d.k. Disagree Agree d.k.
disagree
The mind is the result of the action of the spirit,
or of the soul, on the brain
15 49
36 18
51
31
“State of mind” is a reflection of the brain state 
in a given moment
48 40
12 48
27
25
If there are ways to study brain activity, the
mind can be studied through them
22 56
22 50
34
16
The mind is a product of the working of the
brain
43 45
11 72
22
6
Without a brain, consciousness is not possible
77 12
11 82
8
10
Intuition is a “special sense” that cannot be 
explained by the brain
24 44
32 25
39
36
Individuals are not responsible for behaviour
associated with a developmental difference in
brain function
14 55
31
Table 1 Responses of trainee teachers to assertions of subjective opinion about the
mind-brain relationship and learner agency, the former shown with responses of the
graduate sample of the public studied by Hurculano-Houzel (2002) for comparison.
 

Page 35
Neuroscience Literacy   35 
Trainee teachers
Public (from Hurculano-
Houzel, 2002)
Agree
d.k. disa
gree
agree
d.k. disag
ree
One’s environment can influence hormone production and, 
in turn, personality (C)
61
30
8
64
We use our brains 24 hours a day (C)
89
5
6
92
To learn how to do something, it is necessary to pay
attention to it (C).
43
13 43
73
Learning occurs through modification of the brain’s neural 
connections(C)
50
44
6
30
Performance in activities such as playing the piano
improves as a function of hours spent practising (C)
78
12 10
82
It is with the brain, and not the heart, that we experience
happiness, anger, and fear (C)
76
11 13
97
Hormones influence the body’s internal state, and not their
personality (I)
25
34 42
24
Memory is stored in the brain much like as in a computer.
That is, each memory goes into a tiny piece of the brain (I)
36
38 26
41
Memory is stored in networks of cells distributed
throughout the brain (C)
50
42
8
60
Keeping a phone number in memory until dialling,
recalling recent events & distant experiences, all use the
same memory system (I)
12
45 44
49
When we sleep, the brain shuts down (I)
0
7 93
80

Page 36
Neuroscience Literacy   36 
Table 2 Responses of trainee teachers to a selection of the general assertions
(C=correct assertion, I=incorrect assertion) about the brain intended to assess levels of
neuroscience literacy. Results of the Hurculano-Houzel survey of those members of
public who had been educated at graduate level are provided for comparison
(Hurculano-Houzel, 2002), with blank cells where results were not reported.
*see text for discussion of the correctness, or otherwise, of this assertion
 
Learning is not due to the addition of new cells to the brain
(C*)
52
35 13
82
Brain activity depends entirely on the external
environment: with no senses stimulated, we don’t see, hear 
or feel anything (I)
11
22 66
33
Emotional brain processes interrupt those brain processes
involved with reasoning (I)
69
23
8
70
Cognitive abilities are inherited and cannot be modified by
the environment or by life experience (I)
3
10 87
91
We mostly only use 10% of our brains(I)
52
38 10
48

Page 37
Neuroscience Literacy   37 
 
Trainee teachers
Agree d.k
.
disagree
Children are less attentive after sugary drinks and snacks (I)
63 24
13
Omega 3 supplements do not enhance the mental capacity of children in the
general population (C)
23 54
23
Environments that are rich in stimulus improve the brains of pre-school children (I)
89 10
1
Individuals learn better when they receive information in their preferred learning
style (e.g. visual, auditory, kinaesthetic) (I)
82 11
7
Short bouts of co-ordination exercises can improve integration of left and right
hemispheric brain function (I)
65 31
4
Regular drinking of caffeinated soft drinks reduces alertness (C)
33 45
22
Differences in hemispheric dominance (left brain, right brain) can help explain
individual differences amongst learners (I)
60 35
5
Learning problems associated with developmental differences in brain function
cannot be remediated by education (I)
9 41
49
There are no critical periods in childhood after which you can’t learn some things, 
just sensitive periods when it’s easier (C)
55 30
15
Vigorous exercise can improve mental function (C)
63 29
8
Individual learners show preferences for the mode in which they receive
information (e.g. visual, auditory, kinaesthetic) (C)
79 16
5
Exercises that rehearse co-ordination of motor-perception skills can improve
literacy skills(I)
35 56
9
Production of new connections in the brain can continue into old age (C)
63 25
12
Extended rehearsal of some mental processes can change
the shape and structure of some parts of the brain (C)
62 31
6

Page 38
Neuroscience Literacy   38 
 
Table 3.3 Responses of trainee teachers to a selection of assertions drawn from
educational neuromyths (C=correct assertion based on scientific evidence, I=incorrect
assertion, or an assertion for which there is no scientific evidence). 
 
Drinking less than 6-8 glasses of water a day can cause the brain to shrink (I)
18 39
43

Page 39
Neuroscience Literacy   39 
FIGURES
0
10
20
30
40
Education
Genes
Home
Other
Fig 1 The mean percentage contribution to educational outcomes that 158 trainee 
secondary teachers attributed to education, genes, home environment and other. 
 
This document was added to the Education‐line collection on 15 October 2009