Jump to content

Akuntsu language: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
No edit summary
Content in this edit is translated from the existing Portuguese Wikipedia article at pt:Língua acuntsú; see its history for attribution
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Endangered Tupian language of Brazil}}
{{Short description|Endangered Tupian language of Brazil}}
{{Use dmy dates|date=August 2020}}
{{Use dmy dates|date=August 2020}}
{{Expand Portuguese|Língua acuntsú|date=July 2021}}
{{Infobox language
{{Infobox language
| name = Akuntsu
| name = Akuntsu

Revision as of 11:26, 31 October 2024

Akuntsu
Native toBrazil
EthnicityAkuntsu people
Native speakers
4 (2016)[1]
Tupian
Language codes
ISO 639-3aqz
Glottologakun1241
ELPAkuntsú

Akuntsu is a Tupian language of Brazil.[2] Peaceful contact with the Akuntsu people was only made in 1995; they had been massacred by cattle ranchers in the 1980s.[3]

It is considered unlikely that the Akuntsu language or culture will survive following the deaths of the tribe's remaining members.[4] For this reason several observers have described the tribe as the victims of genocide.[5] The neighbouring Kanoê have been similarly reduced in number through contact with settlers,[6] as were the people of a man recently encountered living alone in the Igarapé Omerê reserve who is apparently the sole survivor of his tribe.[7][8]

Classification

Akuntsu is a Tupian language belonging to the Tupari subfamily. It is related to Tuparí, Kepkiriwát, Makuráp, Mekéns, Waratégaya, and Wayoró. Of these, it is most closely related to Mekéns, sharing approximately 79% of its vocabulary. [9]

Portuguese family tree of the Tupian languages

Phonology

Akuntsu consonants are as follows:

Akuntsu consonants
Bilabial Alveolar Palatal Velar
Plosive Voiceless p t k kw
Voiced b d g
Affricate
Nasal m n ŋ
Flap ɾ w
Approximant w j

The velar nasal cannot occur word-initially.

There are 5 vowels.

Front Central Back
Close i ɨ
Mid e (ɛ) o (u)
Open a

In general, Akuntsu syllables can be formed with a single vowel (V), as in /i’top/ (“look”); a consonant and a vowel (CV), for example in /taˈɾa/ (“wide”); or even one vowel and two consonants (CCV, VCC or CVC), see /ˈhat/ (“snake”), /ˈkwini/ (“fork”) and /oˈajt/ (“butt”). Of these, CV is the most common.[10]

Morphology

There are four open classes of words in Acuntsú (nouns, adverbs, adjectives and verbs) and five closed classes (pronouns, postpositions, idiophones, interjections and particles).

Nouns

Nouns refer to concrete entities (such as epapap, “moon”). They can assume syntactic functions of arguments of verbs and postpositions, as well as being determinants or determinants in nominal determination relations and constituting the nuclei of nominal predicates.

Relational inflection

Relational inflection occurs through prefixes that inflect the noun. The determinant of the nouns is the immediately preceding syntactic expression.

Unlike Tuparí and Makuráp, both also from the Tuparí family, where mutually exclusive prefixes mark the contiguity and non-contiguity of determiners, Akuntsu distinguishes between nouns that combine with the ø- allomorph and those that combine with the t allomorph. - (which mark the contiguity of the determiner, non-contiguity being an unmarked case).[11]

Causal inflection

Two suffixes determine causal inflection in Acuntsú: the locative -po and the determinative -et. The suffix -et gives nouns a specific and determined character. It is known to exist in the forms -t after a vowel and -et after a consonant.

Derivation

In Acuntsú there are suffixes that modify nouns (-tin, which attenuates them, and -atʃo, which intensifies them) and one that forms them. There is also the suffix -kʷa, which in addition to forming a verb does so by bringing the idea of ​​continuity and repetition of the action.

Composition and reduplication

Another way to form names is the composition of nominal themes, such as the formation of ororope (“clothes”) from ororo (“cotton”) and pe (“coating”). The reduplication of the last syllable is also observed in Akuntsu, as in several other languages ​​of the Tupi trunk and indicates plural, as between pi (“foot”) and pipi (“feet”).

Verbs

Verbs in Acuntsú are divided into transitive and intransitive verbs. In general, they differ in that only transitive verbs have objects and, in Acuntsú in particular, there are certain limitations regarding the suffixes that each verb can take.

Both, however, adopt the thematic suffix -a (in the case of verbs ending in /a/, -ø is used, and for verbs ending in /o/, it is replaced by -a - as in "ko", the verb "to eat", which becomes "ka"), which marks the verb in the most diverse constructions, independently of other inflections.

Derivation

Occurring through the addition of prefixes, suffixes and reduplication, the main role of derivation in verbs is to nominalize them or change their transitivity. For nominalization, the suffix -pa stands out, which determines circumstance (atʃo - “to wash”, becomes atʃopa - “washer”), and the affix -i-, which only occurs in transitive verbs and forms a noun corresponding to the object resulting from the verb. An example is the formation of imi (“dead”) from mi (“to kill”). The change in transitivity involves a causative structure. It can be obtained by adding the prefix mõ-, as in kara - mõkara (“to fall” - “to make [object] fall”) or the suffix -ka as in morã - morãka (“to jump” - “to make [object] jump”).

Tense, mood, and aspect

Unlike other languages ​​of the Tupari family, which have a marker for the past, the only clear temporal information is given by the projective marker “kom”, so that the language clearly distinguishes only the “future” and the “non-future”.

The marker kom occurs at the end of sentences and indicates that an event will occur in the future, generally a certain event. An example would be tekarap ita kom (“the rain will come”).

There are three other very important indicators of aspect, namely the suffix -ɾa, which indicates habitualness, the morpheme ekʷa, which indicates iteration, and the suffix -kʷa, which indicates repetition/continuity of the action. Examples are:

  1. apara koara kom. (“I will eat bananas as usual”)
  2. eneme ekʷa. (“You will run many times”)
  3. Konibú imi ekʷa. (“Konibú hunted them”)
  4. oaawkʷa. (“I yawned continuously”)

A peculiarity is that ekʷa, after an intransitive verb, represents an iteration of the action (2), while its presence after a transitive verb indicates an iteration of the object (3).

Reduplication is also an important process for the verbal aspect, occurring in the last syllable of the root, and also indicating iteration/repetition (for example, oøanam ãpika - oøanam ãpipika [“I pull my head” - “I pull and pull my head repeatedly”]).

As for the mood, Akuntsu uses the indicative mood, which has no explicit marking in the language, and is therefore the unmarked case, in addition to the imperative and interrogative moods. The imperative mood is evidenced by the suffix -tʃo, as in idaratʃo (“unfold it”). There is, however, the possibility that the imperative appears without marking (iko, “eat”) The interrogative mood is marked by rising intonation and is divided between polar questions, which require affirmative or negative answers, and content questions, in which the answer concerns a specific point.

Auxiliaries

There are auxiliary verbs in Akuntsu, which express dynamic or static conditions of the subject and contribute to the aspect and directionality of the verb, in addition to being able to present information about the action not included in the semantics of the main verb. They appear at the end of the sentence (with the exception of the morpheme tʃe, also observed before the main verb (1)) and modify the entire construction, and can also occur as main verbs when alone. They are:

standing ãm
seated
lying down toa
in motion ko
coming tʃe
going ka

These markers contribute to aspect by expressing an ongoing event simultaneously with that of the main verb, as in mapi ata kom iko (“he is going to get the arrow”).

Adverbs

Adverbs form a small open class of Akuntsu. Although they are not morphologically distinct from the adjective class, adverbs differ in syntax, since they cannot act as arguments of verbs. The function of adverbs is to add to the semantics of verbs, and they can occur before or after the verb they modify. The process of reduplication is important in adverbs, since it indicates an intensification or reiteration of the characteristic described. Reduplication itself can perform an adverbial function.

Adjectives

There are few adjectives in Acuntsú. They refer to abstractions such as sensations, colors, states, etc. (like akop, “hot”). They modify nouns and can be determined in nominal determination relations, as well as syntactically exercising the function of nuclei of nominal predicates.

Pronouns

Pronouns constitute a closed subclass and are divided into demonstrative and personal pronouns. Among personal pronouns, there are two series: series I encodes both the subject of nominal predicates and the agent of transitive predicates (see section 7.2). They can also act as the nucleus of adverbial phrases (when modified by the locative suffix) or as the emphatic subject of intransitive verbs. They are:

Person Singular Plural Clitic
1 on, one kitʃe (inclusive) otʃe (exclusive) o- ki- (incl.), otʃe- (excl.)
2 en, ene jat e- jat-
3 te kejat i-/t-, te- (referring to a previous subject)

The second person plural is only used when a 3rd person not being referred to is included. Clitic pronouns mark the determiner of nouns, objects of transitive verbs and subjects of intransitive verbs.

Pronome demonstrativo Tradução/referente
eme this/near the speaker
this/near the speaker (sitting)
jȇrom that/far from the speaker and close to the listener
(te)ike away from the speaker and listener, approximately visible

Numerals

There are only two numerals in Akuntsu: kite (“one”) and tɨɾɨ (“two”). To indicate larger numbers, the number two is reduplicated (tɨɾɨ tɨɾɨ is “four”, for example). The numeral can appear before or after the noun it quantifies, but in studies of the language so far, it occurs before the noun more commonly. The numeral kite, in some contexts, can also mean “alone” (oɾẽbõ kite - “I am alone”).

Postpositions

This class includes terms that appear after nouns and express some specific semantic notion. It is approximately equivalent to prepositions in languages ​​such as English and Portuguese, but appears after the terms it determines due to the syntactic structure of Akuntsu

etʃe diffuse locative ek etʃe ka (home locative go) go to the house
ete relative/comitative ta itek ete/en baj ete that is [concerning] his house/is with the buriti
pi Inessive on tek pi tʃopa see the inside of the house
na Translative ikop na he is red

Particles

Particles constitute an closed class of words that can indicate evidentiality, demarcate the focus of a sentence, act as conjunctions, perform adverbial functions and even indicate existence.

Syntax

Noun phrase

In Akuntsu nominal phrases, determined nouns are preceded by their respective determiners. They can also be modified by numerals, which can precede or follow the nouns they modify. There is no gender inflection of any kind, nor is there any number inflection in the noun.

Genitive constructions

The genitive constructions of Akuntsu are based on the combination of nouns in which the determiner precedes the determined, as in øpi, where pi is “foot” and ø- is a prefix that determines the relational inflection.

From this, nouns can be divided into a class of dependent or inalienable nouns, generally referring to parts of a whole or elements whose existence is related to something or someone (examples are ep [“leaf”] and op [“father”]), and independent or alienable nouns, usually plants, animals or elements of nature (e.g. waʔi [“stone”] and ororo [“cotton”]).

While among inalienable nouns genitive constructions are indispensable, among alienable nouns they only occur through mediators, i.e. the nominalizer -i- and the use of kinship names.

The first strategy is to nominalize transitive verbs, forming constructions such as øiko (“my food”) from the verb ko (“to eat”). The other strategy involves a historical aspect of the Akuntsu language: since the Indians had their relatives killed (see section 2.1), the women of the group raised their animals as their children. Thus, kinship names mediate genitive constructions involving independent names of animals. An example is wako Tʃaroj ømempit (“the jacu is the son of Txarúi”), where a standard genitive construction occurs in ømempit (“woman’s son”) associated with the alienable name wako (“jacu”).

Verb phrase

Like nominal phrases, verbal phrases are composed of a verb determined by a determiner that precedes it. An important distinction occurs between intransitive verbs, which receive clitic pronouns to indicate their subject, and transitive verbs, which use independent personal pronouns as subjects according to specific rules.

An example of a verbal phrase with an intransitive verb is oakata, where o- is a clitic pronoun for the 1st person singular that functions as the subject of the verb akata (“to fall”), forming the phrase “I fell”. For transitive verbs there is the presence of one or sometimes even two objects and only one clitic can accompany the verb.

Order and alignment

The default word order of Akuntsu is Subject-Object-Verb.[12] As for alignment, Acuntsú is considered an ergative-absolutive language. This is because the subject of intransitive verbs and the object of transitive verbs occur with the same treatment (in the form of clitic pronouns preceding the verb - absolutive case), while the subjects of transitive verbs occur with another treatment (separately in the sentence - ergative case). However, when the transitive sentence has the name of an inanimate element as the object, the ergative-absolutive alignment can be neutralized.

Interrogatives

Interrogative constructions in Akuntsu are divided into polar and content-based. The former require yes or no answers and are marked by rising intonation. The latter generally occur with particles that begin the sentence (with the exception of “ne”, which occurs at the end) and specify the objective of the question. These particles are:

aɾop who/what/for what/whose aɾop teimi? (“what is he going to hunt?”)
taɾa what/who taɾa ijã? (“what is this?”)
ãka how/when ãka tetʃeta? (“how is he going away?”)
ẽɾom where ẽɾom itʃoka ne? (“where he can build [this]?”)
ne Hypothetical construction enibõ ne? (“Is this for the network?”)

Vocabulary

Loanwords

With the expansion of contact between the Akuntsus and FUNAI groups that do not speak the language, in addition to idiophones, Acuntsú speakers incorporated words from the Kanoê language and Portuguese into their vocabulary, in order to make themselves understood by non-speakers.[13] Examples include:

Loan from Portuguese Portuguese Translation Loan from Kanoê Translation
[ma'hadu] machado axe [uˈɾɛ] pig
[ˈdʒuka] açúcar sugar [tʃeˈɾe] see
[baˈkãw] facão machete [kãˈni] child
[piˈtʃi] peixe fish [iˈwɛ] pain
[mãdʒiˈuka] mandioca cassava [kuˈni] water
[aˈhoj] arroz rice [muˈkujẽ] sleep

References

  1. ^ Maraña, Lati (6 June 2016). "Leader and last ever shaman of tiny Amazon tribe dies in Brazil". Survival International.
  2. ^ Aragon 2014.
  3. ^ Watson, Fiona (13 October 2009). "We're watching an extinction in a lifetime". The Independent. Archived from the original on 15 October 2009. Retrieved 8 March 2011.
  4. ^ "Akuntsu: The future". Survival International. Archived from the original on 21 December 2010. Retrieved 8 March 2011.
  5. ^ Vincent Carelli (Director) (2009). Corumbiara: They Shoot Indians, Don't They? (in Portuguese). Vídeo nas Aldeias.
  6. ^ Instituto Socioambiental (ISA). "Introduction > Kanoê". Povos Indígenas no Brasil. Retrieved 8 March 2011.
  7. ^ Survival International (9 December 2009). "Last survivor of uncontacted Amazon tribe attacked". Retrieved 8 March 2011.
  8. ^ Strange, Hannah (11 December 2009). "'Man in the Hole', lone survivor of Amazon tribe massacre, escapes ranchers' bullets". The Sunday Times. Archived from the original on 29 June 2011. Retrieved 8 March 2011.
  9. ^ Galucio, A. V., & Nogueira, A. F. (2011, October). Comparative study of the Tupari branch of the Tupi family: contributions to understanding its historical development and internal classification. In Memorias del V Congreso de Idiomas Indígenas de Latinoamérica (Vol. 6, No. 8). Austin: Univ. Tex. Austin.
  10. ^ Couto, Fábio Pereira; Isidoro, Edineia Aparecida (30 July 2018). "Evidências Acústicas da Laringalização Vocálica na Língua Tuparí". Revista Brasileira de Linguística Antropológica (in Portuguese). 10 (1): 59–74. doi:10.26512/rbla.v10i1.19054. ISSN 2317-1375.
  11. ^ ARAGON, Carolina Coelho. Fonologia e aspectos morfológicos e sintáticos da língua akuntsú. 2008. Dissertation (Masters in Linguistics) - Departmetn of Linguistics, Portuguese and Classical Languages, University of Brasília, Brasília, 2008..
  12. ^ Aragon 2014, p. 21.
  13. ^ Aragon, Carolina Coelho. «Stylistic and social variations in the discourse of Akuntsú speakers». Federal University of Mato Grosso. Polyfonia. 25 (38.1): 90-103. Retrieved April 12, 2021.

Further reading

  • Aragon, Carolina Coelho (2014). A grammar of Akuntsú, a Tupían language (Ph.D. thesis). University of Hawaii at Manoa. hdl:10125/101050.