User:Zynna.lynn/OurSandbox
Normality (behaviour)
[edit]Normality (normalcy) is the state of being normal. Behaviour can be normal for an individual (intrapersonal normality) when it is consistent with the most common behaviour for that person. "Normal" is also used to describe when someone's behaviour conforms to the most common behaviour in society. Definitions of normality vary by person, time, place, and situation - it changes along with changing societal standards and norms. Normal behaviour is often only recognized in contrast to abnormality. In it's simplest form, normality is seen as good while abnormality is seen as bad.[1] Someone being seen as "normal" or "not normal" can have social ramifications, including being included, excluded or stigmatized by larger society.
Although it is difficult to define normality, since it is a flexible concept, the existence of these ramifications also makes it an important definition. The study of what is normal is called normatology - this field attempts to develop an operational definition distinguishing between normality and abnormality (or pathology). The general question of 'what is normal' is discussed in many fields, including philosophy, psychology and sociology. The most comprehensive attempt to distinguish normality from abnormality comes from clinical psychology, in the Diagnostics and Statistics Manual. The DSM shows how normality is dependent on situation, how it changes throughout history and how it often involve value judgements.
Definitions of Normality
[edit]Normality has been functionally and differentially defined by a vast number of disciplines, so there is not one single definition.
Mathematics
[edit]In general, 'normal' refers to a lack of significant deviation from the average. The word normal is used in a more narrow sense in mathematics, where a normal distribution describes a population whose characteristics centers around the average or the norm. When looking at a specific behaviour, such as the frequency of lying, a researcher may use a Gaussian bell curve to plot all reactions, and a normal reaction would be within one standard deviation, or the most average 68.3%. However, this mathematical model only holds for one particular trait at a time, since, for example, the probability of a single individual being within one standard deviation for 36 variables would be one in a million. [2] .
Durkheim
[edit]The French sociologist Émile Durkheim indicated in his Rules of the Sociological Method that it was necessary for the sociological method to offer parameters to distinguish normality from pathology or abnormality. He suggested that behaviors or "social facts" which are present in the majority of cases are normal, and exceptions to that behavior indicate pathology[3]. Durkheim's model of normality further explained that the most frequent or general behaviors, and thus the most normal behaviors, will persist through transition periods in society. Crime, for instance, exists under every society through every time period, and so should be considered normal[4]. There is a two-fold version of normality; behaviors considered normal on a societal level may still be considered pathological on an individual level. On the individual level, people who violate social norms, such as criminals, will invite a punishment from others in the society.
Influence of Social Norms
[edit]Individuals' behaviours are guided by what they perceive to be society's expectations and their peers norms. People measure the appropriateness of their actions by how far away they are from those social norms. However, what is perceived as the norm may or may not actually be the most common behaviour. In some cases of pluralistic ignorance, most people falsely believe the social norm is one thing, but in fact very few people hold that view.
When people are made more aware of a social norm, particularly a descriptive norm (a norm describing what is done), their behaviour changes to become closer to that norm. The power of these norms can be harnessed by social norms marketing, where the social norm is advertised to people in an attempt to stop extreme behaviour, such as binge drinking. However, people at the other extreme (very little alcohol consumption) are equally likely to change their behaviour to become closer to the norm, in this case by increasing alcohol consumption (an undesirable effect). Instead of using descriptive norms, more effective social norms marketing may use injunctive norms. Instead of describing what behaviour is most commonly done, an injuctive norm is what is approved or disapproved of by society. When individuals become aware of the injunctive norm, only the undesirable extremes will change their behaviour (by decreasing alcohol consumption) without the boomerang effect of under-indulgers increasing their drinking.[5]
The social norms that guide people are not always normal for everyone. Behaviours that are abnormal for most people may be considered normal for a subgroup or subculture. For example, normal college student behaviour may be to party and drink alcohol, but for a subculture of religious students, normal behaviour may be to go to church and pursue wholesome activities. Subcultures may actively reject "normal" behaviour, instead replacing society norms with their own. [6]
Regulation of Normality
[edit]A disharmony exists between a virtual identity of the self and a real social identity, whether it be in the form of a trait or attribute. If a person does not have this disharmony, then they are described as normal. A virtual identity can take many definitions, but in this case a virtual identity is the identity that a person mentally creates that conforms to societal standards and norms, it may not represent how they actually are, but it represents what they believe is the typical "normal" person. A real social identity is the identity that a person actually has in their society, or is perceived by themselves or others, to have. If these two identities have differences between each other, there is said to be disharmony. An individual may monitor and adapt their behaviour in terms of others' expected perceptions of the individual, which is described by the social psychology theory of self-presentation[7] . In this sense, normality exists based on societal norms, and whether someone is normal is entirely up to how they view themselves in contrast to how society views them. While trying to define and quantify normality is a good start, all definitions confront the problem of whether we are even describing an idea that even exists since there are so many different ways of viewing the concept.[8]
Measuring Normality
[edit]Many difficulties arise in measuring normal behaviours- biologists come across parallel issues when defining normality. One complication which arises regards whether 'normality' is used correctly in everyday language [9]. People say “This heart is abnormal” if only a portion of it is not working correctly, yet it may be inaccurate to include the entirety of the heart under the abnormal description. There can be a difference between the normality of the structure and function of a body part. Similarly, a behavioural pattern may not conform to social norms, but still be effective and non-problematic for that individual. Where there is a dichotomy between appearance and function of a behaviour, it may be difficult to measure its normality. This is applicable when trying to diagnose a pathology and is addressed in the DSM.
Relativity of Normality
[edit]What is viewed as normal can change dependent on both timeframe and environment. Normality can be viewed as “an endless process of man’s self-creation and his reshaping of the world”[10]. Within this idea, it is possible to surmise that normality is not an all-encompassing term, but simply a relative term based around a current trend in time. With statistics, this is likened to the thought that if the data gathered provides a mean and standard deviation, over time these data that predict “normalness” start to predict or dictate it less and less since the social idea of normality is dynamic. This is shown in studies done on behavior in psychology and sociology where behavior in mating rituals or religious rituals can change within a century in humans, showing that the “normal” way that these rituals are performed shift and a new procedure becomes the normal one.
As another example, understandings of what is normal sexual behaviour varies greatly across time and place. In many countries, perceptions on sexuality are largely becoming more liberal, especially views on the normality of masturbation and homosexuality. Social understanding on normal sexual behaviour also varies greatly country by country - countries can be divided into categories of how they approach sexual normality, as conservative, homosexual-permissive, or liberal. The United States, Ireland and Poland have more conservative social understanding of sexuality among university students, while Swedish students considered a wider variety of sexual acts as normal. Although some attempts have been made to define sexual acts as normal, abnormal, or indeterminate, these definitions are time-sensitive. Gayle Rubin's 1980s model of sexual 'normality' was comprehensive at the time but has since become outdated as society has liberalized[11] .
Since normality shifts in time and environment, the mean and standard deviation are only useful for describing normality from the environment from which they are collected.
Intrapersonal Normality
[edit]Most definitions of normality consider interpersonal normality, the comparison between many different individual's behaviours to distinguish normality from abnormality. Intrapersonal normality looks at what is normal behaviour for one particular person (consistency within a person) and would be expected to vary person-to-person.[12] A mathematical model of normality could still be used for intrapersonal normality, by taking a sample of many different occurrences of behaviour from one person over time. Also like interpersonal normality, intrapersonal normality may change over time, due to changes in the individual as they age and due to changes in society (since society's view of normality influences individual peoples' behaviour).
It is most comfortable for people to engage in behaviour which conforms to their own personal habitual norms. When things go wrong, people are more likely to attribute the negative outcome on any abnormal behaviour leading up to the mishap. After a car crash, people may say "if only I didn't leave work early", blaming the crash on their actions which were not normal.[13] This counterfactual thinking particularly associates abnormal behaviour with negative outcomes.
Neuronal and Synaptic Normality
[edit]Hebbian associative learning and memory maintenance depends on synaptic normalization mechanisms to prevent synaptic runaway [14]. Where synaptic runaway describes overcrowding of dendritic associations, which reduce sensory or behavioural acuteness proportional to the level of synaptic runaway. Synaptic/neuronal normalization refers to synaptic competition, where the prosper of one synapse may weakening the efficacy of other nearby surrounding synapses with redundant neurotransmission.
Animal dendritic density greatly increases throughout waking hours despite intrinsic normalization mechanisms as described as above. The growth rate of synaptic density is not sustained in a cumulative fashion. Without a pruning state, the signal to noise ratio of CNS mechanism would not be able to operate with maximum effectiveness, and learning would be detrimental to animal survival. Neuronal and synaptic normalization mechanisms must operate so positive association feedback loops to not become rampant while constantly processing new environmental information[14].
Some researchers speculate that the slow oscillation (nREM) cycles of animal sleep constitute an essential ‘re-normalization’ phase. The re-normalization occurs from cortical large amplitude brain rhythm, in the low delta range (0.5-2Hz), synaptically downscaling the associations from the wakeful learning state [15]. Only the strongest associations survive the pruning from this phase. This allows retention of salient information coding from the previous day, but also allows more cortical space and energy distribution to continue effective learning subsequently after a slow-wave oscillation episode of sleep [15]
Also, organisms tend to have a normal biological developmental pathway as a central nervous system ages and/or learns. Deviations for a species normal development frequently will result in behaviour disfunction, or death, of that organism.
Effects of Labelling
[edit]When people do not conform to the normal standard, they are often labelled as sick, disabled, abnormal, or unusual, which can lead to marginalization or stigmatization. Most people want to be normal and strive to be perceived as such, so that they can relate to society at large. Without having things in common with the general population, people may feel isolated among society.[16] The "abnormal" person feels like they have less in common with the "normal" population, and others have difficulty relating to things that they have not experienced themselves. Additionally, abnormality may make others uncomfortable, further separating the abnormally labelled individual.
Since being normal is generally considered an ideal, there is often pressure from external sources to conform to normality, as well as pressure from people's intrinsic desire to feel included. For example, families and the medical community will try and help disabled people live a 'normal' life. However, the pressure to appear normal, while actually having some deviation, creates a conflict - sometimes someone will appear normal, while actually experiencing the world differently or struggling. When abnormality makes society feel uncomfortable, it is the exceptional person themselves who will laugh it off to relieve social tension. A disabled person is given normal freedoms, but may not be able to show negative emotions. Lastly, society's rejection of deviance and the pressure to normalize may cause shame in some individuals. Abnormalities may not be included in an individual's sense of identity, especially if they are unwelcome abnormalities.
When an individuals' abnormality is labelled as a pathology, it is possible for that person to take on both elements of the sick role or the stigmatization that follows some illnesses. Mental illness, in particular, is largely misunderstood by the population and often overwhelms other's impression of the patient.
Clinical Normality
[edit]Applying normality to clinically depends on the field an situation a practitioner is in. In the broadest sense, clinical normality is the idea of uniformity of physical and psychological functioning across individuals. Normality, and abnormality, can be characterized statistically Related to the previous definition, statistically normality is usually defined it in terms of a normal distribution curve, with the so called ‘normal zone’ commonly accounting for 95.45% percent of all the data. The remaining 4.55% will lie split outside of two standard deviations from the mean. Thus any variable case that lies outside of two deviations from the mean would be considered abnormal. However, the critical value of such statistical judgments may be subjectively altered. For example the critical value could be reduced to ±3 SD, or 0.3%, to classify abnormality. Increasing the critical value in that manner would also increase the chances of a type II error, which is referred to clinically as a 'false positive.' The problems associated with clinical false positives are described under 'Critisms of DSM.' It should also be noted that it is in fact normal for a population to have a proportion of abnormals. The presence of abnormals is important because it is necessary to define what 'normal' is, as normality is a relative concept[17]. So at a group, or macro level, of analysis; abnormalities are normal given a demographic survey, but at an individual level abnormal individuals are seen as being deviant in someway that needs to be corrected. Statistical normality is important in determining demographic pathologies. When a variable rate, such as virus spread within a human population, exceeds it's normal infection rate then preventative or emergency measures can be introduced. It is often impractical to apply statistical normality to diagnose individuals. Symptom normality is the current, and assumed most effective, way to assess patient pathology. Psychiatric normality, in a broad sense, states that psychopathology are disorders that are deviations from normality [18].
DSM
[edit]Normality, as a relative concept, is intrinsically involved with contextual elements. As a result, clinical disorder classification has particular challenges in discretely diagnosing ‘normal’ constitutions from true disorders. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) is the psychiatric profession’s official classification manual of mental disorders since its first published version DSM-I in by the APA, 1952. As the DSM evolved into it's current version, DSMTR-IV, there have been numerous conflicts in proposed classification between mental illness and normal mentality.
Most of this difficulty stems from the DSM's ambiguity of natural contextual stressor reactions versus individual dysfunction. There are some key progressions along the DSM history that have attempted to integrate some aspects of normality into proper diagnosis classification. It should be noted that as a diagnostic manual for classification of abnormalities, all DSM’s have been bias towards classifying symptoms as disorders by emphasizing symptomatic singularity. The result is an encompassing misdiagnosis of possible normal symptoms, appropriate as contextually derived[19].
DSM-II
[edit]The second version of the DSM, DSM II, could not be effectively applied because of it’s vague descriptive nature. Psychodynamic etiology was a strong theme in classifying mental illnesses. The applied definitions became idiosyncratic, stressing individual unconscious roots. This made applying the DMV unreliable across psychiatrists[19]. No distinction between abnormal to normal was established.
Evidence of the classification ambiguity were punctated by the Rosenhan experiment of 1972. This experiment demonstrated that the methodology of psychiatric diagnosis could not effectively distinguish normal from disordered mentalities. DSM-II labelled ‘excessive’ behavioral and emotional response as an index of abnormal mental wellness to diagnose some particular disorders [20]. ‘Excessiveness’ of a reaction implied alternative normal behaviour which would have to include a situational factor in evaluation. As an example; a year of intense grief from the death of a spouse may be a normal appropriate response. To have intense grief for twenty years would be indicative of a mental disorder. As well, to grieve intensely over the loss of a sock would also not be considered normal responsiveness and indicate a mental disorder. The consideration of proportionality to stimuli was a perceived strength in psychiatric diagnosis for the DSM-II [19].
Another characteristic of the DSM-II systemization was that it classified of homosexuality as a mental disorder. Thus, homosexuality was psychiatrically defined a pathological deviation from ‘normal’ sexual development. Homosexuality was later replaced in the 7th edition of DSM-II, instead categorized as a ‘Sexual orientation disturbance.’ The intent was to have a label that applied only to those homosexual individuals who were bothered by their sexual orientation[21]. In this manner homosexuality would not be viewed as an atypical illness [21]. Only if it was distressing would homosexuality be classified as a mental illness [20]. However, the DMS-II did not explicitly state that any homosexuality was normal either. This stigma lasted into DSM-II until it was reformed entirely from DSM classifications in 1986.[20][19].
DSM-III
[edit]DSM-III was a best attempt to credit psychiatry as a scientific discipline, from the opprobrium resulting from DSM-II[18]. A reduction in the psychodynamic etiologies of DSM-II spilled over into a reduction symptom etiology altogether. Thus, DSM-III was a specific set of definitions for mental illnesses, and entities more suited to diagnostic psychiatry, but which annexed response proportionality as a classification factor. The product was that all symptoms, whether normal proportional response or inappropriate pathological tendencies, could both be treated as potential signs of mental illness [18].
DSM-IV
[edit]DSM-IV explicitly distinguishes mental disorders and non-disordered conditions. A non-disordered condition results from, and is perpetuated by, social stressors. Included in DSM-IV’s classification is that a mental disorder “must not be merely an expectable and culturally sanctioned response to a particular event, for example, the death of a loved one. Whatever its original cause, it must currently be considered a manifestation of a behavioral, psychological, or biological dysfunction in the individual” (American Psychiatric Association 2000:xxxi) This had supposedly injected normality consideration back into the DSM, from its removal from DSM-II. However, it has been speculated that DSM-IV still does not escape the problems DSM-III faced, where psychiatric diagnoses still include symptoms of expectable responses to stressful circumstances to be signs of disorders, along with symptoms that are individual dysfunctions [19]. The example set by DSM-III, for principally symptom-based disorder classification, has been integrated as the norm of mental diagnostic practice [19].
Criticisms of Normality Diagnostics
[edit]Since the advent of DSM-III, the subsequent editions of the DSM have all included a heavy symptom based pathology diagnosis system. Although there have has been some attempts to incorporate environmental factors into mental and behavioural diagnostics, many practitioners and scientists believe that the most recent DSM's are misused. The sympton bias makes diagnosing quick and easier allowing for practitioners to increase their cliental because symptoms can be easier to classify and deal with than dealing with life or event histories which have evoked what may be a temporary and normal mental state in reaction to a patients environmental circumstances. Thus, a high rate of false positives will result in resource depletion for patients who may not need such a high level of acute or chronic mental care, as other true positive abnormal individuals. The easy-to-use manual not only has increased the perceived need for more mental health care, stimulating funding for mental health care facilities, but also has had a global impact on marketing strategies. Many pharmaceutical commercial ads list symptoms such as fatigue, depression, or anxiety. However, such symptoms are not necessary abnormal, and are appropriate responses to such occurrences as the loss of a loved one. The targets of such ads in such cases do not need medication, and can naturally overcome their grief, but with such an advertising strategy pharmaceutical companies can greatly expand their marketing[19].
See Also
[edit]References
[edit]- ^ Bartlett, Steven James (2011). Normality Does Not Equal Mental Health: The Need to Look Elsewhere for Standards of Good Psychological Health. New York: Praeger. p. xi. ISBN 031339931X, 9780313399312.
{{cite book}}
: Check|isbn=
value: invalid character (help) - ^ Modis, Theodore (March 2007). "The normal, the natural, and the harmonic". Technological Forecasting and Social Change. 74 (3): 391–398. doi:10.1016/j.techfore.2006.07.003. Retrieved December 5.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|accessdate=
(help)CS1 maint: date and year (link) - ^ Durkheim, Émile (1982). Rules of Sociological Method. New York: Free Press. p. 272. ISBN 978-0029079409.
- ^ Jones, Robert Alun (1986). mile Durkheim: An Introduction to Four Major Works. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. pp. 60–81.
- ^ Schultz, P. Wesley (2007). "The Constructive, Destructive, and Reconstructive Power of Social Norms" (PDF). Psychological Science. 18 (5): 429–434. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01917.x. PMID 17576283.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ Magolda, Peter (January 2008). "Students Serving Christ: Understanding the Role of Student Subcultures on a College Campus". You Have Full Text Access to This ContentAnthropology & Education Quarterly. 32 (2): 138–158.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link) - ^ Goffman, Erving (1959). PRESENTATION OF SELF IN EVERYDAY LIFE (PDF). NY: DOUBLEDAY & COMPANY INC.
- ^ Skopalová, Jitka (2010). "Social eviations, Labelling, and Normality" (PDF). Human Affairs. 20 (4): 327–337. doi:10.2478/v10023-010-0034-8. Retrieved December 6, 2011.
- ^ Wachbroit, Robert (1994). "Normality as a Biological Concept". 61 (4). Philosophy Of Science: 579-591. Retrieved December 5, 2011.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help) - ^ Syristová, Eva (2010). A Contribution to Phenomenology of the Human Normality in the Modern Time. Dordrecht: Springer Science + Business Media.
- ^ Morrison, Todd G. (2008). "Canadian university students' perceptions of the practices that constitute "normal" sexuality for men and women". The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality. 17 (4): 161–171. Retrieved December 31, 2011.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Hur, Taekyun; Roese, Neal J.; Namkoong, Jae-Eun (2009). "Regrets in the East and West: Role of intrapersonal versus interpersonal norms". Asian Journal of Social Psychology. 12 (2): 151–156. doi:10.1111/j.1467-839X.2009.01275.x.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help)CS1 maint: date and year (link) - ^ Catellani, Patrizia; Alberici, Augusta I.; Milesi, Patrizia (2004). "Counterfactual thinking and stereotypes: The nonconformity effect". European Journal of Social Psychology. 34 (4): 421–436. doi:10.1002/ejsp.206.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: date and year (link) - ^ a b Arbib, M. A. (1995). The handbook of brain theory and neural networks. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. p. 514.
- ^ a b Tononi, Guilio. "Giulio Tononi: 2011 Allen Institute for Brain Science Symposium". In search of sleep function. Allan Institute for Brain Science. Retrieved 23 December 2011.
- ^ Bridgens, Ruth (5). "Disability and Being 'Normal': A Response to McLaughlin and Goodley: Response". Sociology. 43: 753–761. doi:10.1177/0038038509105419. Retrieved December 10.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|accessdate=
,|date=
, and|year=
/|date=
mismatch (help); Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help) - ^ Thibaut, John W. (1943). "The concept of normality in clinical psychology". Psychological Review. 50 (3): 4–7. doi:10.1037/h0062556.
- ^ a b c Bartlett, Steven (2011). Normality Does Not Equal Mental Health: The Need to Look Elsewhere for Standards of Good Psychological Health. ABC-CLIO.
- ^ a b c d e f g Horwitz, Allan V. (2007). "Transforming Normality into Pathology: The DSM and the Outcomes of Stressful Social Arrangements". Journal of Health and Social Behaviour. 48 (3): 211–222. doi:10.1177/002214650704800301. PMID 17982864.
- ^ a b c Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, Second edition. Washington: American Psychiatric Association. 1968.
- ^ a b "Homosexuality: Proposed Change in DSM-11". The American Psychiatric Association. 6th Printing. 1973.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: date and year (link)