Europe PMC

This website requires cookies, and the limited processing of your personal data in order to function. By using the site you are agreeing to this as outlined in our privacy notice and cookie policy.

Abstract 


The prefrontal cortex (PFC), hippocampus, and amygdala play an important role in emotional health. However, adverse life events (e.g., violence exposure) affect the function of these brain regions, which may lead to disorders such as depression and anxiety. Depression and anxiety disproportionately affect women compared to men, and this disparity may reflect sex differences in the neural processes that underlie emotion expression and regulation. The present study investigated sex differences in the relationship between violence exposure and the neural processes that underlie emotion regulation. In the present study, 200 participants completed a Pavlovian fear conditioning procedure in which cued and non-cued threats (i.e., unconditioned stimuli) were presented during functional magnetic resonance imaging. Violence exposure was previously assessed at four separate time points when participants were 11-19 years of age. Significant threat type (cued versus non-cued) × sex and sex × violence exposure interactions were observed. Specifically, women and men differed in amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus reactivity to cued versus non-cued threat. Further, dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC) and inferior parietal lobule (IPL) reactivity to threat varied positively with violence exposure among women, but not men. Similarly, threat-elicited skin conductance responses varied positively with violence exposure among women. Finally, women reported greater depression and anxiety symptoms than men. These findings suggest that sex differences in threat-related brain and psychophysiological activity may have implications for mental health.

Free full text 


Logo of nppharmLink to Publisher's site
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022 Dec; 47(13): 2221–2229.
Published online 2022 Aug 27. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-022-01430-1
PMCID: PMC9630543
PMID: 36030316

Sex-related differences in violence exposure, neural reactivity to threat, and mental health

Associated Data

Supplementary Materials

Abstract

The prefrontal cortex (PFC), hippocampus, and amygdala play an important role in emotional health. However, adverse life events (e.g., violence exposure) affect the function of these brain regions, which may lead to disorders such as depression and anxiety. Depression and anxiety disproportionately affect women compared to men, and this disparity may reflect sex differences in the neural processes that underlie emotion expression and regulation. The present study investigated sex differences in the relationship between violence exposure and the neural processes that underlie emotion regulation. In the present study, 200 participants completed a Pavlovian fear conditioning procedure in which cued and non-cued threats (i.e., unconditioned stimuli) were presented during functional magnetic resonance imaging. Violence exposure was previously assessed at four separate time points when participants were 11–19 years of age. Significant threat type (cued versus non-cued) × sex and sex × violence exposure interactions were observed. Specifically, women and men differed in amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus reactivity to cued versus non-cued threat. Further, dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC) and inferior parietal lobule (IPL) reactivity to threat varied positively with violence exposure among women, but not men. Similarly, threat-elicited skin conductance responses varied positively with violence exposure among women. Finally, women reported greater depression and anxiety symptoms than men. These findings suggest that sex differences in threat-related brain and psychophysiological activity may have implications for mental health.

Subject terms: Amygdala, Prefrontal cortex

Introduction

Healthy emotional function depends upon emotion expression and regulation processes mediated by the prefrontal cortex (PFC), hippocampus, and amygdala. Disruption of these emotion processes leads to the development of emotion-related disorders (e.g., anxiety, depression) [13]. Women have higher prevalence of emotion-related disorders [4] and differ from men in emotional processes that are supported by the PFC, hippocampus, and amygdala [5]. The PFC, hippocampus, and amygdala support emotion learning, expression, and regulation processes that are important for healthy emotional function [68]. However, negative life events (e.g., violence exposure) may lead to changes among these brain regions resulting in emotional dysfunction [9, 10]. Therefore, determining how neural function varies with negative life events is important for understanding the relationship between these events and emotional function.

Women in the United States are almost twice as likely as men to meet diagnostic criteria for a mood or anxiety disorder during their lifetime [11, 12]. Sex-related differences in emotion-function may underlie this disparity in the rates of mood and anxiety disorders [1116]. Further, prior work has demonstrated that there are sex-related differences in the brain function that underlies emotional expression and regulation [14, 1719]. Taken together, this prior work suggests that differences in brain function may underlie sex differences in emotion regulation and the subsequent development of depression and anxiety. Therefore, understanding the mechanisms that underlie sex differences in emotional function may offer novel insight into sex disparities among these disorders.

Sex differences in emotional function may be mediated by sex differences in brain function [20]. For example, prior work has found greater PFC and amygdala activity in women than men during negatively-valenced emotion tasks, including the evaluation of threats [19, 21, 22]. Additionally, neural activation elicited by sad facial expressions was lateralized to the left hemisphere in men, but right hemisphere in women, suggesting laterality differences between women and men [23]. Further, women show greater PFC activation and less amygdala deactivation than men during the cognitive reappraisal of emotion [18]. Additionally, prior work has demonstrated greater amygdala and orbitofrontal activity in women, and greater PFC and superior parietal cortex activity in men during the cognitive control of emotion [24]. Although sex differences in neural activity vary across studies, women and men often show differences in the brain regions recruited during emotion-related tasks [2527]. Thus, sex-related differences in mood and anxiety symptoms may reflect sex differences in emotion regulation and expression [18, 19]. Finally, these differences may underlie the higher rates of anxiety and depression observed among women in prior studies [11, 28, 29].

Violence exposure can affect emotional function [30, 31]. Moreover, several studies have demonstrated that neural activity during emotion-related tasks varies with violence exposure [3235]. Specifically, violence exposure during childhood and adolescence is associated with alterations in the PFC, hippocampus, and amygdala response to emotion-related stimuli during childhood and adolescence [3335]. Given that healthy emotional function relies upon the PFC, hippocampus, and amygdala, violence exposure may disrupt the function of these brain regions [9]. For example, those that experience violence are more likely to develop depression and anxiety than those who have not been exposed to violence [36]. The effects of violence exposure may be especially prominent during adolescence as the brain regions underlying emotion regulation continue to develop [37]. Further, sex differences in emotional processes may contribute to differences in the way men and women respond to violence. For example, women who have experienced an assault are twice as likely to develop depression, anxiety, and posttraumatic stress disorder than men [38, 39], and experience more severe post-assault symptoms [40]. Taken together, these prior findings suggest there are sex differences in the effects of violence on emotion regulation processes.

Pavlovian fear conditioning is commonly used to investigate the expression and regulation of emotion. Pavlovian fear conditioning is a procedure in which a neutral stimulus is repeatedly paired with an aversive threat (unconditioned stimulus (UCS)). As the relationship between the neutral stimulus and the threat is learned, the neutral stimulus becomes a warning signal (conditioned stimulus (CS)) and anticipatory responses (conditioned responses (CR)) elicited by the warning signal are expressed. Typically, the expression of an anticipatory CR to the warning signal is taken as evidence that learning has occurred. In contrast, the emotional response to the threat (UCS) is typically considered an automatic, reflexive response that does not require learning. However, learning can also be measured by changes in the threat-elicited response (unconditioned response (UCR)). Specifically, behavioral and neural responses to predictable threats are typically smaller than responses to unpredictable threats [32, 4146]. The reduced response amplitude to predictable compared to unpredictable threat is called conditioned UCR diminution, which is mediated by a PFC-hippocampus-amygdala neural circuit [4145, 47]. Investigating the relationship between conditioned UCR diminution and violence exposure may further elucidate the relationship between violence and changes in brain regions that support threat-related emotional processes.

The present study investigated sex differences in the relationship between violence exposure and threat-related neural activity. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was used to study brain activity to cued versus non-cued threat in participants whose violence exposure was previously assessed. We hypothesized that (1) non-cued threats would elicit greater skin conductance responses (SCR) and fMRI signal reactivity within the amygdala, hippocampus, PFC, insula, and inferior parietal lobule (IPL) in accordance with prior work [4145], and that (2) greater violence exposure would be associated with greater differential (i.e., cued versus non-cued threat) activity within these brain regions, and that these effects would be more pronounced in women than men. The present study extends our understanding of the role of sex differences and violence exposure in threat-related emotional function.

Materials and methods

Participants

Two hundred twenty three right-handed volunteers were recruited from the Birmingham, Alabama cohort of the Healthy Passages Study (details on this cohort are provided in the Supplement and Supplementary Tables 1 and 3). Twenty three participants were excluded from the current analyses due to excessive motion or missing data. Therefore, 200 adults (Mean age ± SD = 20.78 ± 1.37, range = 18–23 years, 102 women, 98 men) were included in the present study. Exclusion criteria for this study included standard magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) contraindications (e.g., metallic devices, metallic foreign body, pacemaker), prior head injury, blood/circulation disorders, pregnancy, spinal cord abnormalities, history of seizures, history of psychotic symptoms, and left-handedness. Participants were not excluded for use of prescription medication. All participants provided written informed consent as approved by the University of Alabama at Birmingham Institutional Review Board.

Stimuli

Participants completed a Pavlovian fear conditioning task in which two pure tones (700 and 1300 Hz; 10 s duration) served as warning (CS+) and safety (CS−) signals. The warning signal (CS+) coterminated with a loud (100-dB) white noise (0.5 s duration, 100% pairing rate) that served as the threat (i.e., UCS), while the safety signal (CS−) was presented alone. The threat (UCS) was also presented alone (UCS alone) on some trials. Each stimulus was presented 12 times (18 s inter-trial interval) during each of two fMRI scans (total of 24 CS+, 24 CS−, 24 UCS alone). The tones that served as warning and safety signals were counterbalanced across participants and all stimuli were presented in a pseudo-random order such that no stimulus was presented more than twice consecutively. Analyses assessed differential threat-elicited responses (i.e., UCRs) to the cued threat (i.e., UCS) that coterminated with the CS+ (i.e., CS + UCS) versus the non-cued threat (i.e., UCS alone). The present study focused on differences in the response to cued (CS + UCS) versus non-cued (UCS alone) threat.

SCR

SCR data were sampled (10 kHz) with two radio-translucent electrodes attached to the thenar and hypothenar eminences of the non-dominant hand using a Biopac MP150 MRI compatible physiological monitoring system (Biopac Systems; Goleta, CA) using methods described in prior work [48]. Additional details of SCR analyses are described in the Supplement.

Threat expectancy

Threat expectancy was measured continuously (40 Hz sampling rate) throughout the conditioning procedure using methods described previously [48]. Participants rated their expectancy of threat on a 0 to 100 scale (0 = certain the threat would not be presented; 50 = uncertain the threat would be presented; 100 = certain the threat would be presented). Additional details of the threat expectancy measure are provided in the Supplement.

Violence exposure

Violence exposure was assessed for each participant at four time points, ages 11, 13, 16, and 19 (see Supplement for further information) using the Healthy Passages violence exposure measure [4951]. At each time point, participants reported their exposure to violent incidents in the previous 12 months. Each participant reported how frequently they had been a victim of a threat of violence, physical violence, violence with injury requiring medical treatment, or violence involving a gun or knife. A four-point frequency scale ranging from 0 (Never) to 3 (Many times) was used to score responses. Ratings were averaged within each time point, and then times 1–4 were averaged to reflect cumulative violence exposure.

Depression symptoms

Depression symptoms were assessed at Time 4 using the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Predictive Scales (Major Depression Subscale) [52]. Participants answered 6 dichotomous questions regarding depression symptoms experienced in the past 12 months. Responses were coded 0 (No) or 1 (Yes) and summed to create an index of depression symptoms. The scale had good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.72).

Anxiety symptoms

Anxiety symptoms were assessed at Time 4 using the Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale (Physiological Anxiety subscale) [53]. Participants answered 10 dichotomous questions regarding anxiety symptoms experienced in the past 12 months. Responses were coded 0 (No) or 1 (Yes) and summed to create an index of anxiety symptoms. The scale had good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.65).

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms

PTSD symptoms were assessed at Time 4 using the Child PTSD Symptom Scale [54]. Participants answered 17 questions regarding PTSD symptoms experienced within the past 2 weeks. Responses ranged from 0 (not at all or only 1 time) to 3 (five or more times per week/almost always). The scale had good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.94). Five participants did not complete the questionnaire; therefore, all PTSD analyses include 195 participants.

Functional imaging

FMRI was completed using 3T Siemens Allegra and Prisma scanners. Acquisition parameters and preprocessing methods are described in the Supplement. All analyses were performed using the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) [version: AFNI_18.2.05] software package [55]. A multiple regression analysis was completed to create functional activation maps for each participant using a gamma-variate hemodynamic response function with reference waveforms to model each stimulus (i.e., warning cue, CS+; safety cue, CS−; cued threat, CS + UCS; non-cued threat, UCS alone). Separate regressors were used to account for head motion in six directions and motor activity associated with movement of the joystick. Volumes with excessively high motion (≥3% of voxels greater than five times the median absolute deviation of the timeseries) were censored from the first-level analysis. Percent signal change was used as a measure of the BOLD response amplitude to each stimulus type. The data were then converted to the Talairach and Tournoux stereotaxic coordinate system [56] and resampled to a 1 mm3 resolution.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were completed using IBM SPSS Statistics Version 25 and AFNI [55]. Independent samples t-tests assessed sex differences in violence exposure. Linear mixed effects (LME) models were completed to test the effects of cued versus non-cued threat, sex, violence exposure, and interactions between these terms on SCR, threat expectancy, and fMRI signal responses. A nominal significance threshold was set at p < 0.05 for SCR and threat expectancy analyses. Analyses of covariance were used to determine differences in depression and anxiety between women and men (covariates: family income, neighborhood disadvantage, race (described in the Supplement)). Voxelwise analyses of fMRI data (AFNI’s 3dLME [57]) were completed to assess main effects of sex, violence exposure, and threat type (cued versus non-cued), as well as interaction effects between sex, violence exposure, and threat type. To reduce the number of voxelwise comparisons, group level analyses were performed using a mask to restrict analyses to anatomical regions of interest (PFC, cingulate gyrus, insula, IPL, parahippocampal gyrus (PHG), hippocampus, amygdala) based on prior work [41, 44, 45, 47]. Monte Carlo simulations (AFNI’s 3dClustSim) were performed with the autocorrelation function option to correct for multiple comparisons. A voxelwise threshold of p < 0.005 and a cluster volume threshold of 540 mm3 were used to achieve a corrected significance threshold of p < 0.05. Additional simulations were performed for small volume correction using masks of the parahippocmapal gyrus, hippocampus, and amygdala. Simulations used a voxelwise threshold of p < 0.005 and a cluster threshold of 169 mm3 to achieve a corrected significance threshold of p < 0.05. Scanner, race, family income, and neighborhood disadvantage were used as covariates in all analyses, as these covariates have been found to vary with brain function and violence exposure [32]. Although the abovementioned regions of interest were the focus of the present study, a voxelwise whole brain analysis was also completed (see Supplementary Table 2). AFNI’s 3dClustSim was used as described above to determine the cluster volume threshold (i.e., 608 mm3 with p < 0.005uncorrected) for a corrected significance threshold of p < 0.05.

Secondary analyses

Functional MRI, anxiety, depression, and PTSD

Moderation analyses: secondary analyses were completed to determine whether sex moderated the association between the fMRI signal response to threat type and anxiety, depression, and PTSD symptoms. Data from the main effect of threat type contrast (Table 1) were used as predictor variables. Using PROCESS [58, 59], multiple regression analyses were completed to assess whether sex moderated the association between the neural response to threat and anxiety, depression, and PTSD symptoms. Conditional effects of the neural response to threat and sex on anxiety, depression, and PTSD symptoms were assessed. Race, family income, neighborhood disadvantage, and scanner were included as covariates.

Table 1

Brain regions that show threat-elicited activity.

Main effect of threat typeHemisphereF-statisticTalairach coordinates (x,y,z)Cluster size (mm3)
 dmPFCRight33.167, 31, 3640,575
 dlPFCLeft14.97−29, 4, 561789
Left(2)18.63−45, 18, 421316
Right14.2635, 3, 381361
 InsulaRight36.2031, 20, −54582
Right(2)26.0846, −16, 7783
Left42.22−26, 20, −45584
 PCCRight16.827, −29, 243064
 IPLRight27.0353, −46, 25881
Left24.01−56, −55, 38686
Main effect of sex
 dmPFCRight22.592, 18, 534075
 dlPFCRight14.8722, 47, 28869
 PCCRight15.835, −41, 251438
 IPLLeft13.32−40, −50, 49688
Sex × Threat type
 dlPFCLeft14.18−20, 41, 361017
 Precentral gyrusLeft17.08−45, 12, 72464
 InsulaLeft17.32−37, −11, 01172
Left(2)21.96−45, 14, 15610
Right18.9042, −8, 05911
 Mid CingulateRight19.2814, −16, 421489
 PCCRight16.768, −42, 92466
 IPLLeft14.76−48, −34, 261505
 AmygdalaRight22.1621, −6, −22374
 Parahippocampal gyrusRight16.7123, −23, −25301
Sex × Violence exposure
 dlPFCRight16.7531, 23, 281073
 IPLLeft15.99−42, −48, 47688

Coordinates refer to the location of the peak voxel within each volume of activation. F-statistic values refer to the F value at the peak voxel within each volume of activation. Significance threshold pcorrected < 0.05. Numbers within parentheses in the Hemisphere column reflect a distinct ROI within a particular brain region [e.g., dlPFC Left(2) is the 2nd of two ROIs within the left dlPFC].

dlPFC dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, dmPFC dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, PCC posterior cingulate cortex, IPL inferior parietal lobule.

Results

Violence exposure

No differences were observed in the amount of violence exposure between women and men (women: M = 0.13 ± 0.02, range = 0.00–3.00; men: M = 0.14 ± 0.02, range = 0.00–3.00, t[198] = −0.64, p = 0.52; two-tailed; 95% CI: −0.06–0.03). No sex differences were observed for violence exposure at any time point (Supplementary Table 3).

Depression symptoms

Women (M = 2.38, SEM = 0.16, range = 0.00–6.00) reported more depression symptoms than men (M = 1.73, SEM = 0.16, range = 0.00–5.00), F[1,195] = 9.62, p = 0.002. Among women, violence exposure varied positively with depression symptoms (r = 0.26, p = 0.01), while there was no relationship for men (r = 0.15, p = 0.16); however, these correlations were not different (z = 0.84, p = 0.40). Full results are presented in the Supplement (Supplementary Table 4).

Anxiety symptoms

Women (M = 3.12, SEM = 0.21, range = 0.00–9.00) reported more anxiety symptoms than men (M = 2.51, SEM = 0.21, range = 0.00–9.00), F[1,195] = 4.91, p = 0.03. Among women and men, violence exposure varied positively with anxiety symptoms (women: r = 0.24; p = 0.02, men: r = 0.23, p = 0.02). Full results are presented in the Supplement (Supplementary Table 5).

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms

There were no differences in PTSD symptoms between women (M = 9.69, SEM = 1.15, range = 0.00–51.00) and men (M = 7.72, SEM = 0.90, range = 0.00–37.00), F[1,190] = 1.10, p = 0.30. Among men, violence exposure varied positively with PTSD symptoms (r = 0.27, p = 0.01), while there was no relationship for women (r = 0.11, p = 0.30); however, these correlations were not significantly different (z = 1.14, p = 0.25). Full results are presented in the Supplement (Supplementary Table 6).

SCR

SCR data from 85 participants were excluded from analyses due to poor signal quality/equipment malfunction (n = 13) or non-responsiveness (n = 72). Poor signal quality was defined as excessive noise in the signal that prevented identification of stimulus-evoked SCRs. Equipment malfunction was defined as no signal observed during scanning due to a problem with equipment set up or function. Participants exhibiting no responses greater than 0.05 µS were considered non-responders. The LME analysis revealed a significant main effect of violence exposure (F[1,109] = 11.37, p < 0.001). Specifically, SCR varied positively with violence exposure (r = 0.19, p = 0.04). There was also a significant sex × violence exposure interaction (F[1,109] = 18.95, p < 0.001). Specifically, SCR to threat varied positively with violence exposure among women (r = 0.52, p < 0.001) but not for men (r = −0.07, p = 0.60). Since the LME analysis did not reveal a significant difference in threat type, a follow-up paired samples t-test was conducted to determine whether there were differences in SCR by threat type. SCR to non-cued threat (M = 0.43, SEM = 0.03) was larger than to cued threat (M = 0.39, SEM = 0.03; Mean difference: 0.03, t[1,114] = −2.56, p = 0.01; two-tailed; 95% CI: 0.01–0.06), consistent with prior work [43, 44, 46, 47].

Threat expectancy

The LME revealed a significant main effect for threat type (F[1,193] = 90.57, p < 0.001) such that expectancy was greater for cued (M = 70.46, SEM = 1.26) than non-cued (M = 55.27, SEM = 0.98) threat. No other significant effects were observed.

Functional MRI

The LME analysis of fMRI data revealed main effects of threat type and sex, as well as threat type × sex and violence exposure × sex interactions. The fMRI results showed a main effect of threat type, such that the fMRI response was greater to non-cued than to cued threat within the dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC), dorsomedial PFC (dmPFC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), insula, and IPL consistent with prior research (Table 1) [41, 4345, 60]. We also observed a main effect of sex within the dlPFC, dmPFC, IPL, and PCC (Table 1). Specifically, threat-elicited activity within these regions was greater in women than men.

There was a significant threat type × sex interaction within the right amygdala (Supplementary Table 7 and Fig. 1a). Specifically, a greater fMRI signal response was observed within the amygdala to non-cued threat in women (M = 0.08, SEM = 0.02) than men (M = 0.03, SEM = 0.02; t[1,193] = 2.19, p = 0.03). Further, the response to cued threat was smaller for women (M = 0.02, SEM = 0.02) than men (M = 0.09, SEM = 0.02) within the amygdala (t[1,193] = −3.03, p = 0.003; Fig. 1a). There was also a significant threat type × sex interaction within the right PHG (Supplementary Table 7 and Fig. 1b). PHG activity to non-cued threat was larger in women (M = 0.11, SEM = 0.02) than men (M = 0.05, SEM = 0.02; t[1,193] = 2.42, p = 0.02). Further, PHG activity to cued threat was smaller in women (M = 0.07, SEM = 0.02) than men (M = 0.14, SEM = 0.02; t[1,193] = −2.13, p = 0.04, Fig. 1b). Finally, there was a significant threat type × sex interaction within the left dlPFC, left IPL, left precentral gyrus, bilateral insula, right mid cingulate gyrus, and right PCC (Supplementary Table 7).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 41386_2022_1430_Fig1_HTML.jpg
Amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus response to threat.

a A threat type × sex interaction was observed within the right (a) amygdala and (b) parahippocampal gyrus. Women (black bars) exhibited a smaller response to cued threat (CS + UCS) than men (gray bars). In contrast, women exhibited a larger response to non-cued (UCS Alone) threat than men. *p < 0.05.

A significant sex × violence exposure interaction was observed within the right dlPFC and left IPL (Table 1). Among women, violence exposure varied positively with the fMRI response within the dlPFC and IPL. In men, violence exposure varied negatively with the fMRI response within the dlPFC, while no relationship was observed within the IPL (Fig. 2).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is 41386_2022_1430_Fig2_HTML.jpg
Left inferior parietal lobule (IPL) and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) response to threat.

A sex × violence exposure interaction was observed within the left IPL. a Women (black scatterplot) demonstrated a positive relationship (r = 0.37, p < 0.01) between violence exposure and the IPL response to threat, while b men (gray scatterplot) demonstrated no relationship (r = −0.12, p = 0.24). A similar sex × violence exposure interaction was also observed within the right dlPFC. c Women (black scatterplot) demonstrated a positive relationship (r = 0.31, p < 0.01) between violence exposure and the dlPFC response to threat, while d men (gray scatterplot) demonstrated a negative relationship (r = −0.22, p = 0.04).

Functional MRI, anxiety, depression, and PTSD

Secondary analyses were completed to determine whether sex moderated the association between the fMRI response to threat type and anxiety, depression, and PTSD symptoms. Moderation analyses were not corrected for multiple comparisons; therefore, results should be interpreted with caution. Anxiety: sex did not moderate the association between the neural response to threat (cued or non-cued) and anxiety (Supplementary Table 8). Depression: sex moderated the association between the right insula response to non-cued threat and depression. Specifically, there was a positive association between the insula response and depression symptoms in men, but not women (Supplementary Table 9). Although the interaction between the left IPL response to non-cued threat and depression symptoms was significant, the slopes for men and women were not significant (Supplementary Table 9). Sex did not moderate the association between cued threat and depression (Supplementary Table 9). PTSD: sex moderated the association between the right IPL response to cued threat and PTSD symptoms. Specifically, the IPL response varied positively with PTSD symptoms in women, but not men (Supplementary Table 10). Sex also moderated the association between left IPL response to cued threat and PTSD symptoms. Specifically, the left IPL response varied positively with PTSD symptoms in women, but not men (Supplementary Table 10). Sex did not moderate the association between non-cued threat and PTSD symptoms (Supplementary Table 10).

Discussion

The present study investigated sex differences in the relationship between violence exposure and the neural response to threat. We found that women showed greater threat-elicited activity within the dlPFC, dmPFC, IPL, and PCC than men. Further, we found that women demonstrated greater amygdala and PHG responses to non-cued threat than men, while the response to cued threat was greater in men than women. We also found that dlPFC and IPL activity varied positively with violence exposure in women. In contrast, activity within these regions showed a negative (dlPFC) or no (IPL) relationship with violence exposure in men. Further, women reported greater depression and anxiety symptoms than men, while no sex differences were observed in PTSD symptoms. Additionally, anxiety symptoms varied positively with violence exposure in women and men; however, depression symptoms and violence exposure varied positively in women only. Lastly, PTSD symptoms varied positively with violence exposure in men only. These results suggest that adolescent violence exposure may differentially impact the neural function that supports threat-related emotion processes in women and men, and varies with depression, anxiety, and PTSD symptoms in both women and men. Findings from the present study provide novel insight into the neural circuitry underlying sex differences in emotional function.

The present study demonstrated sex differences in neural reactivity to cued and non-cued threat. Specifically, we found greater dlPFC, cingulate, IPL, and precentral gyrus responses to non-cued threat in women than men, while no differences were observed in responses to cued threat. Further, insula reactivity to cued threat was greater in men than women, while no differences were observed to non-cued threat. The dlPFC, cingulate, and IPL are important for threat-related learning and the cognitive control of emotion [41, 44, 61, 62]. Further, prior work has demonstrated greater dlPFC and IPL activity under uncertain task conditions [6365] and linked activity within these brain regions to anxiety [66, 67]. Additionally, amygdala and PHG activation to non-cued threat was greater in women than men. In contrast, amygdala and PHG activation to cued threat was greater in men than in women (Fig. 1). Differences observed in these responses to threat may also be linked to threat predictability. For example, prior work indicates that unpredictable threats are perceived as more aversive than predictable threats [68, 69], which is consistent with other research that has demonstrated larger autonomic responses to unpredictable than predictable threats [4143, 45, 47, 70]. Similar findings have been observed within neuroimaging data. Specifically, greater brain activity to unpredictable threats has been linked to anxiety about the uncertainty of the threat [68]. In the present study, women showed greater amygdala and PHG activation to non-cued threat, which may reflect greater affective responding when threats are uncertain. Prior work indicates that affective responses to uncertainty are larger in those with anxiety disorders [71], which occur more frequently in women [11]. In fact, women in the present study reported greater anxiety symptoms than men. Overall, the findings from the present study suggest there is greater recruitment of brain regions important for both cognitive control (e.g., dlPFC, IPL) and affective responding (e.g., amygdala and PHG) in women when threats are unpredictable. Thus, the current findings suggest that the dlPFC, IPL, amygdala, and PHG response to unpredictable threat is greater in women which may be linked to symptoms of anxiety.

The present study also demonstrated sex differences in the relationship between violence exposure and the neural response to threat (Fig. 2). Specifically, women who experienced greater violence exposure showed a greater response to threat within the dlPFC and IPL. In contrast, men demonstrated a negative relationship between violence exposure and dlPFC activity and no relationship between violence exposure and IPL activity. The dlPFC and IPL are important components of the fronto-parietal network and modulate threat-elicited brain activity [42, 45, 72, 73]. Further, greater dlPFC and IPL activation is associated with increased emotion regulation and cognitive control [62, 72, 74]. Thus, the present results suggest that women who have been exposed to higher levels of violence exposure may recruit brain regions that are important for the cognitive control of emotion during threat compared to men who show the opposite pattern. Furthermore, women demonstrated a positive relationship between violence exposure and SCR to threat, while men demonstrated no relationship. Taken together, these findings suggest that the brain and psychophysiological response to threat in women, but not in men, may be linked to their exposure to violence.

Results from the present study demonstrate sex-related differences in neural reactivity to threat. These findings suggest that neural reactivity to threat varies with violence exposure. Prior studies demonstrate that as the brain continues to develop during adolescence (i.e., puberty), trauma (e.g., violence exposure) may alter the neural response to threat due to increased sensitivity to violence and other forms of trauma [7579]. Consistent with the suggestion that adolescents may show greater sensitivity to adverse events, prior work demonstrates greater psychophysiological reactivity to laboratory stressors as adolescents age and reach pubertal maturation [8082]. Thus, violence exposure during this period of development may result in greater physiological reactivity. Further, prior research has found a greater risk of anxiety, depression, and PTSD throughout both adolescence and adulthood in those exposed to greater prepubertal and pubertal maltreatment and violence [83, 84]. Taken together, findings from previous studies suggest that violence exposure during adolescence may enhance the response to threat which may influence future mental health symptoms.

Results from secondary analyses demonstrate that sex moderated the association between the neural response to threat and depression and PTSD symptoms. First, among men, the right insula response to non-cued threat varied positively with depression symptoms, while there was no association among women, and no associations for cued threat. The insula is important for integrating external stimuli with internal bodily states, and supports the processing of emotion-related stimuli [8587]. Prior studies have shown a positive association between the insula response to negative stimuli and depression symptoms [88]. Results from the present study suggest that greater depression symptoms in men are associated with greater activity in brain regions important for emotion processing when threat is uncertain. Additionally, we found that among women, the association between PTSD symptoms and bilateral IPL responses to cued threat varied positively while there was no association for men, and no associations for non-cued threat. Prior studies have demonstrated a positive association between PTSD symptom severity and IPL responses to emotional stimuli [89]. Given that the IPL is important for cognitive control of emotion, women with higher PTSD symptoms may recruit the IPL to a greater extent than women with lower PTSD symptoms, to modulate emotional responses when threats are predictable. Overall, these findings suggest there are sex-related differences in brain regions that underlie emotion expression. Further, the present findings align with prior studies that suggest that men and women may use different brain regions to regulate emotions [2527]. While these findings provide novel information regarding differences in brain regions that underlie emotion expression in response to threat in women and men, these results were not corrected for multiple comparisons, and thus, should be interpreted with caution.

Strengths and limitations

The present study has several strengths. Specifically, the present study includes a large community sample with approximately equal numbers of men and women, increasing statistical power and generalizability. However, the present study is not without limitation. For example, the present study is cross sectional; thus, we cannot determine whether violence exposure influences neural activity or whether the relationships observed were due to preexisting differences. Thus, future studies should implement longitudinal neuroimaging designs to determine whether violence exposure impacts the neural response to threat over time. Additionally, prior work suggests that reproductive hormones and menstrual cycle phase may impact the brain function that supports fear learning processes [17, 90]. Follow-up analyses in the present study suggest that the neural response to threat did not vary as a function of hormonal medication (Supplementary Tables 11 and 12). However, future studies should assess the presence of reproductive hormones and menstrual cycle phase when examining sex differences in neural function. Finally, cultural factors (e.g., socialization, coping style) were not assessed in the present study, but may modulate sex differences in emotion expression [91, 92]. Thus, future studies should assess cultural factors when examining sex differences in the emotional response to threat.

Conclusion

The current study found sex differences in the neural response to threat, as well as sex differences in the relationship between violence exposure and threat-elicited neural activity. Although prior studies have investigated sex differences in neural function during Pavlovian fear conditioning [9395] this study is the first to investigate sex differences in the neural and psychophysiological response to threat that are linked to violence exposure. Additionally, results suggest that there are sex differences in the neural response to threat uncertainty which may be linked to sex-related differences in symptoms of anxiety. Overall, findings from the present study suggest that sex differences in the neural response to threat may underlie sex differences in emotional function.

Supplementary information

Supplement(100K, docx)

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Peyton Thetford for assistance with data collection.

Author contributions

SM, MAS, MNE, STE, and DCK conceptualized and designed the project. HED, NGH, AMG, KHW, MDW, DRH, DCK, and SM have full access to the data. HED, NGH, AMG, KHW, and MDW collected data for the project. HED, DRH, and NGH analyzed the data. HED, DRH, NGH, DCK, and SM interpreted the data. HED, DRH, NGH, and DCK revised the work critically for important intellectual content. SM, MAS, MNE, STE, DCK, HED, NGH, AMG, KHW, and MDW had final approval of the version to be published. All authors agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Institutes of Health (Grant number: R01 MH098348). The original Healthy Passages study, from which the participants in the present study were recruited, was approved by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the institutional review boards of the study sites and supported by cooperative agreements (CCU409679, CCU609653, CCU915773, U48DP000046, U48DP000057, U48DP000056, U19DP002663, U19DP002664, and U19DP002665). The funding sources had no role in the analysis or interpretation of the data, writing of the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41386-022-01430-1.

References

1. Donegan NH, Sanislow CA, Blumberg HP, Fulbright RK, Lacadie C, Skudlarski P, et al. Amygdala hyperreactivity in borderline personality disorder: implications for emotional dysregulation. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:1284–93. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
2. Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, Lane R. Neurobiology of emotion perception II: implications for major psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:515–28. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
3. Eaton NR, Keyes KM, Krueger RF, Balsis S, Skodol AE, Markon KE, et al. An invariant dimensional liability model of gender differences in mental disorder prevalence: evidence from a national sample. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121:282. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
4. Strine TW, Mokdad AH, Balluz LS, Gonzalez O, Crider R, Berry JT, et al. Depression and anxiety in the United States: findings from the 2006 behavioral risk factor surveillance system. Psychiatr Serv. 2008;59:1383–90. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
5. Wrase J, Klein S, Gruesser SM, Hermann D, Flor H, Mann K, et al. Gender differences in the processing of standardized emotional visual stimuli in humans: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci Lett. 2003;348:41–5. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
6. Davidson RJ, Irwin W. The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends Cogn Sci. 1999;3:11–21. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
7. Ochsner KN, Bunge SA, Gross JJ, Gabrieli JD. Rethinking feelings: an FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:1215–29. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
8. Quirk GJ, Beer JS. Prefrontal involvement in the regulation of emotion: convergence of rat and human studies. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2006;16:723–7. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
9. Sherin JE, Nemeroff CB. Post-traumatic stress disorder: the neurobiological impact of psychological trauma. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2011;13:263–78. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
10. McCrory EJ, De Brito SA, Sebastian CL, Mechelli A, Bird G, Kelly PA, et al. Heightened neural reactivity to threat in child victims of family violence. Curr Biol. 2011;21:R947–8. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
11. Kessler RC, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Wittchen HU. Twelve‐month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2012;21:169–84. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
12. Wade TJ, Cairney J, Pevalin DJ. Emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence: national panel results from three countries. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41:190–8. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
13. Deng Y, Chang L, Yang M, Huo M, Zhou R. Gender differences in emotional response: inconsistency between experience and expressivity. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0158666. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
14. Lungu O, Potvin S, Tikàsz A, Mendrek A. Sex differences in effective fronto-limbic connectivity during negative emotion processing. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;62:180–8. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
15. Thayer JF, Rossy LA, Ruiz-Padial E, Johnsen BH. Gender differences in the relationship between emotional regulation and depressive symptoms. Cogn Ther Res. 2003;27:349–64. [Google Scholar]
16. Kelly MM, Tyrka AR, Anderson GM, Price LH, Carpenter LL. Sex differences in emotional and physiological responses to the Trier Social Stress Test. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2008;39:87–98. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
17. Sacher J, Neumann J, Okon-Singer H, Gotowiec S, Villringer A. Sexual dimorphism in the human brain: evidence from neuroimaging. Magn Reson Imaging. 2013;31:366–75. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
18. McRae K, Ochsner KN, Mauss IB, Gabrieli JJD, Gross JJ. Gender differences in emotion regulation: an fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal. Group Process Intergr Relat. 2008;11:143–62. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
19. Domes G, Schulze L, Böttger M, Grossmann A, Hauenstein K, Wirtz PH, et al. The neural correlates of sex differences in emotional reactivity and emotion regulation. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31:758–69. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
20. Cahill L. Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:477–84. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
21. Stevens JS, Hamann S. Sex differences in brain activation to emotional stimuli: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:1578–93. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
22. McClure EB, Monk CS, Nelson EE, Zarahn E, Leibenluft E, Bilder RM, et al. A developmental examination of gender differences in brain engagement during evaluation of threat. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:1047–55. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
23. Lee TMC, Liu H-L, Hoosain R, Liao W-T, Wu C-T, Yuen KSL, et al. Gender differences in neural correlates of recognition of happy and sad faces in humans assessed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci Lett. 2002;333:13–6. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
24. Koch K, Pauly K, Kellermann T, Seiferth NY, Reske M, Backes V, et al. Gender differences in the cognitive control of emotion: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2744–54. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
25. Shirao N, Okamoto Y, Okada G, Ueda K, Yamawaki S. Gender differences in brain activity toward unpleasant linguistic stimuli concerning interpersonal relationships: an fMRI study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2005;255:327–33. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
26. Thomas M, Gaudeau-Bosma C, Bouaziz N, Andrianisaina PS-K, Benadhira R, Januel D, et al. Gender-related brain activations during an emotional task: an fMRI study. Neuropsychobiology. 2019;78:128–35. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
27. Seo D, Ahluwalia A, Potenza MN, Sinha R. Gender differences in neural correlates of stress‐induced anxiety. J Neurosci Res. 2017;95:115–25. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
28. Seney ML, Sibille E. Sex differences in mood disorders: perspectives from humans and rodent models. Biol Sex Differ. 2014;5:17. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
29. Altemus M, Sarvaiya N, Epperson CN. Sex differences in anxiety and depression clinical perspectives. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2014;35:320–30. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
30. Scarpa A, Fikretoglu D, Luscher K. Community violence exposure in a young adult sample: II. Psychophysiology and aggressive behavior. J Community Psychol. 2000;28:417–25. [Google Scholar]
31. Singer MI, Anglin TM, Yu Song L, Lunghofer L. Adolescents’ exposure to violence and associated symptoms of psychological trauma. JAMA. 1995;273:477–82. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
32. Harnett NG, Wheelock MD, Wood KH, Goodman AM, Mrug S, Elliott M, et al. Negative life experiences contribute to racial differences in the neural response to threat. Neuroimage. 2019;202:116086. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116086. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
33. Jenness JL, Rosen ML, Sambrook KA, Dennison MJ, Lambert HK, Sheridan MA, et al. Violence exposure and neural systems underlying working memory for emotional stimuli in youth. Dev Psychopathol. 2018;30:1517–28. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
34. Hein TC, Goetschius LG, McLoyd VC, Brooks-Gunn J, McLanahan SS, Mitchell C, et al. Childhood violence exposure and social deprivation are linked to adolescent threat and reward neural function. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2020;15:1252–9. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
35. van Rooij SJ, Smith RD, Stenson AF, Ely TD, Yang X, Tottenham N, et al. Increased activation of the fear neurocircuitry in children exposed to violence. Depress Anxiety. 2020;37:303–12. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
36. Kilpatrick DG, Acierno R. Mental health needs of crime victims: epidemiology and outcomes. J Trauma Stress. 2003;16:119–32. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
37. Tottenham N, Galván A. Stress and the adolescent brain: amygdala-prefrontal cortex circuitry and ventral striatum as developmental targets. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;70:217–27. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
38. Fridh M, Lindström M, Rosvall M. Experience of physical violence and mental health among young men and women: a population-based study in Sweden. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:29. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
39. Purtle J, Adams-Harris E, Frisby B, Rich JA, Corbin TJ. Gender differences in posttraumatic stress symptoms among participants of a violence intervention program at a pediatric hospital. Fam Community Health. 2016;39:113–9. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
40. Breslau N, Chilcoat HD, Kessler RC, Peterson EL, Lucia VC. Vulnerability to assaultive violence: further specification of the sex difference in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med. 1999;29:813–21. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
41. Dunsmoor JE, Bandettini PA, Knight DC. Neural correlates of unconditioned response diminution during Pavlovian conditioning. Neuroimage. 2008;40:811–7. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
42. Goodman AM, Harnett NG, Knight DC. Pavlovian conditioned diminution of the neurobehavioral response to threat. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;84:218–24. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
43. Harnett NG, Wheelock MD, Wood KH, Ladnier JC, Mrug S, Knight DC. Affective state and locus of control modulate the neural response to threat. Neuroimage. 2015;121:217–26. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
44. Knight DC, Waters NS, King MK, Bandettini PA. Learning-related diminution of unconditioned SCR and fMRI signal responses. Neuroimage. 2010;49:843–8. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
45. Wood KH, Ver Hoef LW, Knight DC. Neural mechanisms underlying the conditioned diminution of the unconditioned fear response. Neuroimage. 2012;60:787–99. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
46. Knight DC, Lewis EP, Wood KH. Conditioned diminution of the unconditioned skin conductance response. Behav Neurosci. 2011;125:626. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
47. Wood KH, Wheelock MD, Shumen JR, Bowen KH, Ver Hoef LW, Knight DC. Controllability modulates the neural response to predictable but not unpredictable threat in humans. Neuroimage. 2015;119:371–81. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.086. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
48. Knight DC, Wood KH. Investigating the neural mechanisms of aware and unaware fear memory with fMRI. J Vis Exp. 2011;56:e3083. 10.3791/3083. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
49. Mrug S, Loosier PS, Windle M. Violence exposure across multiple contexts: Individual and joint effects on adjustment. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 2008;78:70–84. 10.1037/0002-9432.78.1.70. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
50. Mrug S, Madan A, Windle M. Emotional desensitization to violence contributes to adolescents’ violent behavior. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2016;44:75–86. 10.1007/s10802-015-9986-x. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
51. Eaton DK, Kann L, Kinchen S, Ross J, Hawkins J, Harris WA, et al. Youth risk behavior surveillance-United States, 2005. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2006;55:1–108. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
52. Lucas CP, Zhang H, Fisher PW, Shaffer D, Regier DA, Narrow WE, et al. The DISC Predictive Scales (DPS): efficiently screening for diagnoses. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:443–9. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
53. Reynolds CR, Richmond BO. Revised children’s manifest anxiety scale (RCMAS). Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 1985.
54. Foa EB, Johnson KM, Feeny NC, Treadwell KR. The Child PTSD Symptom Scale: a preliminary examination of its psychometric properties. J Clin Child Psychol. 2001;30:376–84. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
55. Cox RW. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res. 1996;29:162–73. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
56. Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. New York: Thieme; 1988.
57. Chen G, Saad ZS, Britton JC, Pine DS, Cox RW. Linear mixed-effects modeling approach to FMRI group analysis. Neuroimage. 2013;73:176–90. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
58. Hayes AF. PROCESS: a versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling [white paper]. http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf 2012.
59. Hayes AF, Preacher KJ. Conditional process modeling: using structural equation modeling to examine contingent causal processes. In: Hancock GR, Mueller RO, editors. Structural equation modeling: a second course. 2nd ed. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing; 2013.
60. Wood KH, Kuykendall D, Ver Hoef LW, Knight DC. Neural substrates underlying learning-related changes of the unconditioned fear response. Open Neuroimag J. 2013;7:41. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
61. Wood KH, Ver Hoef LW, Knight DC. The amygdala mediates the emotional modulation of threat-elicited skin conductance response. Emotion. 2014;14:693. 10.1037/a0036636. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
62. Ochsner KN, Silvers JA, Buhle JT. Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: a synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012;1251:E1–24. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06751.x. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
63. Huettel SA, Mack PB, McCarthy G. Perceiving patterns in random series: dynamic processing of sequence in prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:485–90. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
64. Huettel SA, Song AW, McCarthy G. Decisions under uncertainty: probabilistic context influences activation of prefrontal and parietal cortices. J Neurosci. 2005;25:3304–11. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
65. Vickery TJ, Jiang YV. Inferior parietal lobule supports decision making under uncertainty in humans. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:916–25. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
66. Basten U, Stelzel C, Fiebach CJ. Trait anxiety and the neural efficiency of manipulation in working memory. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2012;12:571–88. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
67. Grieder M, Homan P, Federspiel A, Kiefer C, Hasler G. Increased anxiety after stimulation of the right inferior parietal lobe and the left orbitofrontal Cortex. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:375. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract]
68. Grupe DW, Nitschke JB. Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: an integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14:488–501. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
69. Schell AM, Grings WW. Judgments of UCS intensity and diminution of the unconditioned GSR. Psychophysiology. 1971;8:427–32. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
70. Knight DC, Nguyen HT, Bandettini PA. The role of the human amygdala in the production of conditioned fear responses. Neuroimage. 2005;26:1193–200. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.020. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
71. Simmons A, Matthews SC, Paulus MP, Stein MB. Intolerance of uncertainty correlates with insula activation during affective ambiguity. Neurosci Lett. 2008;430:92–7. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
72. Sylvester CM, Corbetta M, Raichle ME, Rodebaugh TL, Schlaggar BL, Sheline YI, et al. Functional network dysfunction in anxiety and anxiety disorders. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35:527–35. 10.1016/j.tins.2012.04.012. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
73. Wheelock MD, Sreenivasan KR, Wood KH, Ver Hoef LW, Deshpande G, Knight DC. Threat-related learning relies on distinct dorsal prefrontal cortex network connectivity. Neuroimage. 2014;102:904–12. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
74. Delgado MR, Nearing KI, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA. Neural circuitry underlying the regulation of conditioned fear and its relation to extinction. Neuron. 2008;59:829–38. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
75. Dahl RE. Adolescent brain development: a period of vulnerabilities and opportunities. Keynote address. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1021:1–22. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
76. Jenness JL, Peverill M, Miller AB, Heleniak C, Robertson MM, Sambrook KA, et al. Alterations in neural circuits underlying emotion regulation following child maltreatment: a mechanism underlying trauma-related psychopathology. Psychol Med. 2021;51:1880–9. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
77. Konrad K, Firk C, Uhlhaas PJ. Brain development during adolescence: neuroscientific insights into this developmental period. Dtsch Ärzteblatt Int. 2013;110:425. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
78. Neufang S, Specht K, Hausmann M, Güntürkün O, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Fink GR, et al. Sex differences and the impact of steroid hormones on the developing human brain. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:464–73. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
79. Marusak HA, Martin KR, Etkin A, Thomason ME. Childhood trauma exposure disrupts the automatic regulation of emotional processing. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:1250–8. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
80. Gunnar MR, Wewerka S, Frenn K, Long JD, Griggs C. Developmental changes in hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal activity over the transition to adolescence: normative changes and associations with puberty. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21:69–85. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
81. Stroud LR, Foster E, Papandonatos GD, Handwerger K, Granger DA, Kivlighan KT, et al. Stress response and the adolescent transition: performance versus peer rejection stressors. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21:47–68. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
82. Stroud LR, Papandonatos GD, Williamson DE, Dahl RE. Sex differences in the effects of pubertal development on responses to a corticotropin‐releasing hormone challenge: the Pittsburgh Psychobiologic Studies. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1021:348–51. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
83. Marshall AD. Developmental timing of trauma exposure relative to puberty and the nature of psychopathology among adolescent girls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;55:25–32.e1. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
84. Capretto JJ. Developmental timing of childhood physical and sexual maltreatment predicts adult depression and post-traumatic stress symptoms. J Interpers Violence. 2020;35:2558–82. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
85. Duffau H, Taillandier L, Gatignol P, Capelle L. The insular lobe and brain plasticity: lessons from tumor surgery. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2006;108:543–8. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
86. Menon V. Salience network. In: Toga AW. editor. Brain mapping: an encyclopedic reference, vol. 2, pp. 597–611. Academic Press: Elsevier; 2015.
87. Uddin LQ. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16:55–61. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
88. Lee B-T, Cho SW, Khang HS, Lee B-C, Choi I-G, Lyoo IK, et al. The neural substrates of affective processing toward positive and negative affective pictures in patients with major depressive disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2007;31:1487–92. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
89. White SF, Costanzo ME, Blair JR, Roy MJ. PTSD symptom severity is associated with increased recruitment of top-down attentional control in a trauma-exposed sample. NeuroImage Clin. 2015;7:19–27. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
90. Milad MR, Igoe SA, Lebron-Milad K, Novales J. Estrous cycle phase and gonadal hormones influence conditioned fear extinction. Neuroscience. 2009;164:887–95. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
91. Chaplin TM. Gender and emotion expression: a developmental contextual perspective. Emot Rev. 2015;7:14–21. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
92. Chaplin TM, Hong K, Bergquist K, Sinha R. Gender differences in response to emotional stress: an assessment across subjective, behavioral, and physiological domains and relations to alcohol craving. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32:1242–50. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
93. Merz CJ, Wolf OT, Schweckendiek J, Klucken T, Vaitl D, Stark R. Stress differentially affects fear conditioning in men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:2529–41. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
94. Lebron-Milad K, Abbs B, Milad MR, Linnman C, Rougemount-Bücking A, Zeidan MA, et al. Sex differences in the neurobiology of fear conditioning and extinction: a preliminary fMRI study of shared sex differences with stress-arousal circuitry. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2012;2:7. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
95. Inslicht SS, Metzler TJ, Garcia NM, Pineles SL, Milad MR, Orr SP, et al. Sex differences in fear conditioning in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47:64–71. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Neuropsychopharmacology are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

Citations & impact 


Impact metrics

Jump to Citations

Citations of article over time

Alternative metrics

Altmetric item for https://www.altmetric.com/details/135053743
Altmetric
Discover the attention surrounding your research
https://www.altmetric.com/details/135053743

Article citations

Funding 


Funders who supported this work.

NIA NIH HHS (1)

NIMH NIH HHS (1)

U.S. Department of Health &amp; Human Services | NIH | National Institute of Mental Health (1)

U.S. Department of Health &amp; Human Services | National Institutes of Health (1)