Jump to content

Object–subject word order

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

In linguistic typologyobject–subject (OS) word order, also called O-before-S or patient–agent word order, is a word order in which the object appears before the subject. OS is notable for its statistical rarity as a default or predominant word order among natural languages.[1] Languages with predominant OS word order display properties that distinguish them from languages with subject–object (SO) word order.[2]

The three OS word orders are VOS, OVS, and OSV. Collectively, these three orders comprise only around 2.9% of the world's languages. SO word orders (SOV, SVO, VSO) are significantly more common, comprising approximately 83.3% of the world's languages (the remaining 13.7% have free word order).[3]

Despite their low relative frequency, languages that use OS order by default can be found across a wide variety of families, including Nilotic,[4] Austronesian, Mayan, Oto-Manguean, Chumashan, Arawakan,[5] Cariban, Tupi–Guarani, , Nadahup,[6] and Chonan.[7]

Examples

[edit]

CERT:certainty (evidential):evidentiality

Tzeltal (VOS)[5]

la

PST

y-il

3.SG-see

te'tikil

wild

mut

chicken

ta

in

hamal

forest

te

ART

Ziak-e

Ziak-ART

la y-il te'tikil mut ta hamal te Ziak-e

PST 3.SG-see wild chicken in forest ART Ziak-ART

'Ziak saw a wild bird in the forest.'

Selk'nam (OVS)[7]

Kųlįųt

European

matị-n

kill-CERT.MASC

nị-y

PRES-MASC

Kịyųk

Keyuk

Kųlįųt matị-n nị-y Kịyųk

European kill-CERT.MASC PRES-MASC Keyuk

'Keyuk kills the white man.'

Xavante (OSV)[6]

Toptö

Toptö

wahi

snake

mate

it

ti-tsa

her-bite

Toptö wahi mate ti-tsa

Toptö snake it her-bite

'A snake bit Toptö.'

Properties

[edit]

Ditransitive constructions

[edit]

According to Maria Polinsky (1995), the default order of the subject and the object relative to each other in a given language determines other features of that language's syntax.[2] In particular, languages with default SO order construct ditransitive clauses differently than languages with OS order:

[I]n languages where subject precedes object, causee, recipient, and benefactive precede the patient in double object constructions. [...] Correspondingly, in languages where object precedes subject, causee, recipient, and benefactive follow the patient in double object constructions.

Polinsky 1995, p. 190-191

The patient can also be referred to as the direct object (DO) and the causee/recipient/benefactive as the indirect object (IO).

To demonstrate this principle, Polinsky provides an example sentence from Malagasy, which has V–DO–IO–S order:

h-amp-anasa

FUT-CAUS-wash

an'

ACC

Jaona

John

an'

ACC

Jeanne

Jeanne

Paoly

Paul

h-amp-anasa an' i Jaona an' i Jeanne i Paoly

FUT-CAUS-wash ACC ART John ACC ART Jeanne ART Paul

'Paul will be having Jeanne wash John.'

Notice how in the original Malagasy, John (the direct object) precedes Jeanne (the indirect object), whereas in the English equivalent, Jeanne precedes John. English, unlike Malagasy, has S–V–IO–DO order.

Another example of this phenomenon, from Päri (DO–V–S–IO order):

diɛl-Ø

goat-ABS

áñúth'ì

showed

pònd'-ɛ

boy-ERG

rwʌth-Ø

chief-ABS

diɛl-Ø áñúth'ì pònd'-ɛ rwʌth-Ø

goat-ABS showed boy-ERG chief-ABS

'The boy showed the chief the goat.'

Note that Polinsky's principle does not state anything about the order of the indirect object and the subject relative to each other, hence the difference between Malagasy (IO–S) and Päri (S–IO) in this regard.

Correlation with ergativity

[edit]

Anna Siewierska (1996) suggests that ergative–absolutive alignment is overrepresented in OS languages relative to SO languages. To test this, she measured the occurrence of ergative alignment in two samples (SO vs. OS languages) across three categories: agreement, pronouns, and nouns (since some languages have different alignment systems in different categories). She then calculated the frequency of ergativity in each category relative to the sample.

Word Order Ergative Agreement Ergative Pronouns Ergative Nouns
Number % Non-Neutral[a] % Total Number % Non-Neutral[b] % Total Number % Non-Neutral[c] % Total
SO (n=198) 9 6% 5% 15 16% 9% 29 30% 15%
OS (n=12) 2 22% 16% 3 43% 27% 3 60% 25%

Notably, full noun phrases in the OS sample (but not the SO sample) favor ergative alignment, with a majority (60%) of noun alignment in the OS sample being ergative. This is the only cell of the table with a higher than 50% frequency of ergativity. Even in the other categories, the OS sample consistently has a higher relative frequency of ergativity than the SO sample. However, Siewierska notes that her sample size of 12 OS languages is too small for a significance test.

Siewierska theorizes that, in these languages, ergativity may have arisen from the reanalysis of passive clauses as active transitive clauses. The order of the patient and the agent relative to each other remained during this reanalysis, resulting in unmarked OS word order.[8]

Object-initial word order

[edit]
This Venn diagram divides the three OS word orders into two sets: object-initial and subject-final. OVS is the intersection of both sets.

A notable subset of OS order is object-initial word order, in which the object appears first in the clause. This includes OVS and OSV, but not VOS (which is verb-initial, i.e. the verb appears first in the clause).

In a 1979 study, Desmond C. Derbyshire and Geoffrey K. Pullum reported that predominant object-initial word order only occurs in the Amazonian language area. Amazonian languages with object-initial order include Hixkaryana, Urubu, Apurinã, Xavante, and Nadëb.[6] However, since Derbyshire and Pullum's study, examples of languages with object-initial order have been found outside the Amazon. These include Mangarayi (Australia),[9] Äiwoo (Melanesia),[10] and Päri (Africa).[4] The ancient Mesopotamian language Hurrian frequently utilizes object-initial order (OSV) in its attested writing, although it's debatable whether or not this can be considered the default order of the language.[11]

Typologically, object-initial order can be analyzed as the presence of OS order within an OV structure, since the object comes before both the subject and the verb. OV structure is most commonly found in languages with SOV order, such as Japanese and Turkish. Features generally associated with OV structure include the use of postpositions rather than prepositions (i.e. constructions like "the house in" rather than "in the house" as in English), and placement of the genitive before the noun rather than after, among other features.[12] As for object-initial languages, Edward L. Keenan III (1978) notes that features associated with OV order are present in Hixkaryana, for example.[5]

Matthew Dryer (1997) proposes that in the case of ergative–absolutive languages that display this pattern (such as Mangarayi and Päri), the first constituent in the order is technically not the object but the absolutive, since the conventional notions of "subject" and "object" (best suited to a nominative–accusative paradigm) do not exactly apply.[13] Thus, it may be more appropriate to describe these particular languages as absolutive-initial rather than object-initial.

Statistics

[edit]

The scarcity of OS as a default word order has been observed since at least 1963, when Joseph Greenberg proposed the tendency of subjects to precede objects as his first universal. Other linguists of the 20th century, such as Theo Vennemann in 1973, even stated that true OS languages were not attested at all.[1]

In 2013, Dryer surveyed 1377 languages to determine which word orders are more frequently predominant than others. His findings are summarized in the table below:[3]

Word Order Number Percentage[d]
SOV 565 41.0%
SVO 488 35.4%
VSO 95 6.9%
VOS 25 1.8%
OVS 11 0.8%
OSV 4 0.3%
Unfixed 189 13.7%

On the basis of the SO / OS dichotomy, this table can be divided into two halves: the three SO orders (SOV, SVO, VSO) constitute the more common half of the table, while the three OS orders (VOS, OVS, OSV) constitute the less common half. The two object-initial orders (OVS and OSV) are the rarest of all. Even the least common SO order, VSO, is still significantly more common (6.9%) than all three of the OS orders combined (2.9%).

Hammarström (2016)[14] surveyed the word orders of 5252 languages in two ways: counting the languages directly, and stratifying them by language families. Both of these methods yielded a ranking of the word orders identical to Dryer's ranking, albeit with different percentages:

Word Order No. of Languages Percentage No. of Families Percentage[e]
SOV 2275 43.3% 239 56.6%
SVO 2117 40.3% 55 13.0%
VSO 503 9.5% 27 6.3%
VOS 174 3.3% 15 3.5%
OVS 40 0.7% 3 0.7%
OSV 19 0.3% 1 0.2%
Unfixed 124 2.3% 26 6.1%

The data shows a preference for SO order over OS order among the languages of the world. Linguists have proposed several explanations for this phenomenon.

Theoretical explanations for scarcity

[edit]

Perhaps subjects precede objects because the subject of an action verb denotes the causal agent (as in Dog bites man); putting the subject first mirrors the cause coming before the effect.

Steven Pinker, The Language Instinct (1994), p. 236

Keenan: Relevance Principle

[edit]

Keenan (1978)[5] postulates a Relevance Principle that motivates placing the subject before the object.

The Relevance Principle: The reference of the subject (phrase) determines in part, the relevance of what is said, regardless of what it is, to the addressee.

In a typical sentence, the subject is the same as the topic, i.e. the thing that is being talked about. Thus, in a language that puts the subject first, a listener can immediately determine whether or not the speaker's utterance is relevant to them personally (or relevant to what has already been said). Conversely, a language that postpones the subject will require the listener to process a larger portion of the utterance in order to determine how relevant it is.

Take the following example sentence:

John left the meeting early.

If John is a political candidate the listener is supporting, this sentence is much more relevant to the listener than if John is just a man who is setting up the tables. Thus, the relevance of "left the meeting early" to the listener is dependent upon the relevance of "John".

Derbyshire and Pullum: survivorship bias

[edit]

In their 1979 study, Derbyshire and Pullum suggest that the scarcity of OS languages (and specifically object-initial languages) relative to SO languages may simply be the result of survivorship bias rather than any underlying structural motivation.[f] They argue that the global prevalence of SO order, and SVO in particular, has been amplified by the colonial expansion of the English, French, Spanish, Portuguese, and Dutch empires (all of which speak SVO languages) and the resultant mass language extinction events in the continents which they colonized. Thus, there may have been an unknown number of OS languages which were driven to extinction by colonizing SO languages.[6]

Tily et al.: cognitive bias

[edit]

Tily et al. (2011)[15] performed an experiment with a sample of 285 native English speakers. In the experiment, the participants were taught simple phrases in constructed languages of all six possible word orders, then performed trials in these languages to demonstrate how successfully they had acquired the syntax. The researchers calculated how many of the trials were performed correctly for each word order:

Word Order Correct Trials
SVO 100%
SOV 94%
VSO 91%
OVS 88%
OSV 83%
VOS 74%

The researchers note that these results may be biased by the fact that all the participants are native speakers of English, which has SVO word order. This would explain why the two orders with the poorest performances (OSV and VOS) are those in which none of the three constituents are in the same position as in SVO. However, this English bias does not explain the high accuracy score of SOV in particular, and VSO to a lesser extent, relative to all the OS orders. The researchers hypothesize that there may be a universal cognitive bias in favor of placing agents before patients, but note that this hypothesis has yet to be tested with participants whose native language is not English.

See also

[edit]

OS word orders

Other word order phenomena

Notes

[edit]
  1. ^ i.e. leaving out languages that lack morphosyntactic distinction between subject, agent, and patient in verbal agreement.
  2. ^ i.e. leaving out languages that lack morphosyntactic distinction between subject, agent, and patient in pronouns.
  3. ^ i.e. leaving out languages that lack morphosyntactic distinction between subject, agent, and patient in nouns.
  4. ^ Percentages were not directly mentioned in Dryer's study, but are evident from his reported frequency of each word order relative to the sample size of 1377.
  5. ^ Hammarström included families with no data in his count (58 out of 424 = 13,7%), but did not include them in the list. This explains why the percentages do not sum to 100% in this column.
  6. ^ Derbyshire and Pullum do not use the exact phrase "survivorship bias" but describe the same phenomenon in different words.

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b Hall, Matthew L.; Ferreira, Victor S.; Mayberry, Rachel I. (March 18, 2014). "Investigating Constituent Order Change With Elicited Pantomime: A Functional Account of SVO Emergence". Cognitive Science. 38 (5): 934–972. doi:10.1111/cogs.12105. PMC 4082436. PMID 24641486.
  2. ^ a b Polinsky, Maria (January 1995). "Double Objects in Causatives: Towards a Study of Coding Conflict". Studies in Language. 19 (1): 129–221 [188–191]. doi:10.1075/sl.19.1.05pol.
  3. ^ a b Dryer, Matthew S. (2013). "Order of Subject, Object and Verb". In Dryer, Matthew S.; Haspelmath, Martin (eds.). The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.
  4. ^ a b Andersen, Torben (August 1988). "Ergativity in Päri, a Nilotic OVS Language". Lingua. 75 (4): 289–324. doi:10.1016/0024-3841(88)90008-3.
  5. ^ a b c d Keenan, Edward L. III (1978). "The Syntax of Subject-Final Languages". In Lehmann, Winfred P. (ed.). Syntactic Typology: Studies in the Phenomenology of Language. University of Texas. ISBN 0-292-77545-8. LCCN 78-56377.
  6. ^ a b c d Derbyshire, Desmond C.; Pullum, Geoffrey K. (1979). "Object initial languages". Work Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota Session. 23 (2). doi:10.31356/silwp.vol23.02.
  7. ^ a b Rojas-Berscia, Luis Miguel (2014). A Heritage Reference Grammar of Selk'nam. Nijmegen: Radboud University. p. 89.
  8. ^ Siewierska, Anna (1996). "Word order type and alignment type". Sprachtypologie und Universalienforschung. 49: 149–176.
  9. ^ Merlan, Francesca (1982). Mangarayi. North-Holland: Lingua Descriptive Studies. ISSN 0166-2139.
  10. ^ Ross, Malcolm & Åshild Næss (2007). "An Oceanic Origin for Aiwoo, the Language of the Reef Islands?". Oceanic Linguistics. 46 (2): 456–498. doi:10.1353/ol.2008.0003. hdl:1885/20053. S2CID 143716078.
  11. ^ Pullum, Geoffrey K. (1977). Cole, P.; Sadock, J. M. (eds.). "Word order universals and grammatical relations". Syntax and Semantics. 8: 249–277 [262–264]. doi:10.1163/9789004368866_011. ISBN 978-90-04-36886-6.
  12. ^ Dryer, Matthew S. (2013). "Order of Object and Verb". In Dryer, Matthew S.; Haspelmath, Martin (eds.). The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.
  13. ^ Dryer, Matthew S. (1997). "On the 6-way Word Order Typology" (PDF). Studies in Language. 21 (1): 69–103 [89]. doi:10.1075/sl.21.1.04dry.
  14. ^ Hammarström, H. (2016). "Linguistic diversity and language evolution". Journal of Language Evolution. 1 (1): 19–29. doi:10.1093/jole/lzw002. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0029-2F3E-C.
  15. ^ Tily, Harry J.; Frank, Michael C.; Jaeger, T. Florian (January 2011). The learnability of constructed languages reflects typological patterns. Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. pp. 1364–1369 [1367–1369]. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.208.3381.