Europe PMC

This website requires cookies, and the limited processing of your personal data in order to function. By using the site you are agreeing to this as outlined in our privacy notice and cookie policy.

Abstract 


An association of gestational weight gain (GWG) with offspring cognition has been postulated. We used data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, a United Kingdom prospective cohort (1990 through the present) with a median of 10 maternal weight measurements in pregnancy. These were used to allocate participants to 2009 Institute of Medicine weight-gain categories and in random effect linear spline models. Outcomes were School Entry Assessment score (age, 4 years; n = 5,832), standardized intelligence quotient assessed by Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (age, 8 years; n = 5,191), and school final-examination results (age, 16 years; n = 7,339). Offspring of women who gained less weight than recommended had a 0.075 standard deviation lower mean School Entry Assessment score (95% confidence interval: -0.127, -0.023) and were less likely to achieve adequate final-examination results (odds ratio = 0.88, 95% confidence interval: 0.78, 0.99) compared with offspring of women who gained as recommended. GWG in early pregnancy (defined as 0-18 weeks on the basis of a knot point at 18 weeks) and midpregnancy (defined as 18-28 weeks on the basis of knot points at 18 and 28 weeks) was positively associated with School Entry Assessment score and intelligence quotient. GWG in late pregnancy (defined as 28 weeks onward on the basis of a knot point at 28 weeks) was positively associated with offspring intelligence quotient and with increased odds of offspring achieving adequate final-examination results in mothers who were overweight prepregnancy. Findings support small positive associations between GWG and offspring cognitive development, which may have lasting effects on educational attainment up to age 16 years.

Free full text 


Logo of amjepidLink to Publisher's site
Am J Epidemiol. 2013 Mar 1; 177(5): 402–410.
Published online 2013 Feb 6. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kws239
PMCID: PMC3581073
PMID: 23388581

Associations of Maternal Weight Gain in Pregnancy With Offspring Cognition in Childhood and Adolescence: Findings From the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children

Abstract

An association of gestational weight gain (GWG) with offspring cognition has been postulated. We used data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, a United Kingdom prospective cohort (1990 through the present) with a median of 10 maternal weight measurements in pregnancy. These were used to allocate participants to 2009 Institute of Medicine weight-gain categories and in random effect linear spline models. Outcomes were School Entry Assessment score (age, 4 years; n = 5,832), standardized intelligence quotient assessed by Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (age, 8 years; n = 5,191), and school final-examination results (age, 16 years; n = 7,339). Offspring of women who gained less weight than recommended had a 0.075 standard deviation lower mean School Entry Assessment score (95% confidence interval: −0.127, −0.023) and were less likely to achieve adequate final-examination results (odds ratio = 0.88, 95% confidence interval: 0.78, 0.99) compared with offspring of women who gained as recommended. GWG in early pregnancy (defined as 0–18 weeks on the basis of a knot point at 18 weeks) and midpregnancy (defined as 18–28 weeks on the basis of knot points at 18 and 28 weeks) was positively associated with School Entry Assessment score and intelligence quotient. GWG in late pregnancy (defined as 28 weeks onward on the basis of a knot point at 28 weeks) was positively associated with offspring intelligence quotient and with increased odds of offspring achieving adequate final-examination results in mothers who were overweight prepregnancy. Findings support small positive associations between GWG and offspring cognitive development, which may have lasting effects on educational attainment up to age 16 years.

Keywords: ALSPAC, cognition, gestational weight gain

In humans, brain development occurs primarily in utero and during early infancy, and nutrition during these periods may be important for normal development and later cognition. For example, greater dysregulation of second- and third-trimester maternal lipid metabolism (indicated by higher levels of serum free fatty acids and β-hydroxybutyrate in the blood) is inversely correlated with childhood intelligence quotient (IQ) at age 4 years in offspring of diabetic mothers (1, 2). In diabetic mothers, acetonuria (a signifier of fasting or starvation) during pregnancy is inversely associated with offspring intelligence at age 5 years (3). In contrast, evidence from the Dutch Famine suggests that extreme malnutrition during pregnancy does not affect cognition in offspring at age 19 years (4), though cognitive decline in older age in the same population began earlier in those exposed to famine in utero than in those who were not exposed (5).

These scenarios are extreme and it is unclear whether less drastic undernutrition that affects gestational weight gain (GWG) but does not necessarily cause weight loss is related to offspring cognition (6). To the best of our knowledge, only 1 previous study has assessed the relationship between GWG and offspring cognition. That study found that offspring of women who gained between 2 and 13 kg during pregnancy scored higher on the Raven Colored Progressive Matrices test at age 5 years than did those whose mothers gained less than 2 kg or more than 13 kg, even when adjusting for the mothers’ prepregnancy weight/height ratio (7).

Our aim was to use repeated measures of gestational weight to examine the association of GWG with offspring's School Entry Assessment (SEA) scores at age 4 years, IQ at age 8 years, and school final-examination results at age 16 years. We hypothesized that GWG would be positively associated with outcomes assessed in early life, and that this association may have a long-term impact on school attainment at age 16 years (the legal minimum school leaving age in the United Kingdom when critical examinations are taken to determine whether a student can continue on to higher education) and on future occupation outcomes.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is a prospective, population-based birth cohort study that recruited 14,541 pregnant women resident in Avon, United Kingdom, with expected delivery dates between April 1, 1991, and December 31, 1992 (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/). The study included 13,617 mother-offspring pairs from singleton livebirths who survived to at least 1 year; only these are considered here. We further restricted analyses to women with complete GWG data (n = 12,484) and with term deliveries (between 37 and 44 weeks of gestation, n = 12,449). Figure 1 shows the study participant flow. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the local research ethics committee.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is kws23901.jpg

Study participant flow diagram, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, Bristol, United Kingdom, 1990–Present. GCSE, General Certificate of Secondary Education; GWG, gestational weight gain; IQ, intelligence quotient; SEA, School Entry Assessment.

Gestational weight gain

Maternal GWG was examined in 2 ways. A categorical measure of “less than recommended,” “as recommended,” and “more than recommended” was created, reflecting the 2009 Institute of Medicine (IOM) recommendations (6). The recommended weight gain varies on the basis of prepregnancy body mass index (BMI), which is measured and defined as weight (kg)/height (m)2. The recommended absolute gain (weight in late pregnancy before delivery minus weight prepregnancy or at the start of pregnancy) is 12.5–18 kg for women with a BMI of <18.5; 11.5–16 kg for women with a BMI of 18.5–24.9; 7–11.5 kg for women with a BMI of 25–29.9 and 5–9 kg for women with a BMI of ≥30 (6).We classified women according to these prepregnancy BMI categories to determine whether each subject's GWG was less than, as, or more than recommended. Prepregnancy BMI was based on self-reports of prepregnancy height and weight obtained when women enrolled in the study. Additionally, all pregnancy weight measurements (median, 10 measurements; interquartile range, 8–11 measurements) were used to develop a linear spline multilevel model (with 2 levels, woman and measurement occasion) relating weight to gestational age, with knots at 18 and 28 weeks. This multilevel model was then used to predict each woman's weight at 0 weeks’ gestation (referred to as “prepregnancy weight”) and GWG (per week) from 0 to 18 weeks (early pregnancy, based on a knot point at 18 weeks); from 18 to 28 weeks (midpregnancy, based on knot points at 18 and 28 weeks); and from 28 weeks to delivery (late pregnancy, based on a knot point at 28 weeks). Further details of the multilevel model used to derive these variables are described by Fraser et al. (8) and can be found in the Web Appendix, Web Table 1, and Web Figures 1 and 2 available at http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/. In particular, there is no evidence that the model predicted any better or worse depending on the subject's weight, and thus measurement error in our measures of prepregnancy weight and GWG is likely to be nondifferential across these exposures. Maternal prepregnancy weight and GWG were scaled to be meaningful by examining the variation in offspring outcomes per additional 1 kg of maternal prepregnancy weight and per 400 g of gain per week of gestation (6). Prepregnancy weight predicted from the spline multilevel model was highly correlated with self-reported prepregnancy weight (Pearson's r = 0.94).

Outcomes

SEA scores (based on examinations taken at 4 years of age) were extracted from Local Educational Authority recording. The scores from 0 (low) to 20 (high) are derived from the summation of 4 required skills (language, reading, writing, and mathematics) and assessed by teachers for every child enrolled in the compulsory education system in the United Kingdom.

At 8 years of age, children's cognitive function was measured in the ALSPAC research clinics using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (9). IQs (mean, 100 (standard deviation, 15) points) are derived from these data. The tests comprise 10 subtests (5 verbal and 5 performance subtests) that sum to the verbal IQ and performance IQ. All tests were administered by clinicians who had earned a bachelor's degree in psychology.

General Certificate of Secondary Education (GCSE) results were extracted from the National Pupil Database. GCSEs are examinations taken by secondary school children aged 16 years (the minimum age at which children are legally allowed to finish formal education in the United Kingdom; they are thus the final school examinations taken by all children in the United Kingdom). Results were coded as 3 separate binary measures. The category indicating “high achievement” represents a grade of A* (highest possible grade) or A on 9 or more GCSEs. The category indicating “adequate final-examination results” represents a grade ranging between A* and C (lowest passing grade) on 5 or more GCSEs, including those in English and mathematics. (This measurement is relevant because it is the requirement to continue to higher education and also a requirement for many semiskilled jobs.) The category indicating “low achievement” represents no passing grades on any GCSEs.

Other variables

Information on maternal age, mode of delivery (cesarean or vaginal delivery), and the child's sex was obtained from obstetrical records. On the basis of questionnaire responses, maternal education level was assessed and divided into 5 categories: 1) Certified Secondary Education, a lower level of attainment than ordinary (O-level) examinations taken at age 16 years; 2) vocational training; 3) ordinary (O-level) examinations; 4) advanced (A-level) examinations; and 5) university degree.

Information on the highest occupation of parental figures in the household was recorded from questionnaire responses collected in late pregnancy (≥32 weeks’ gestation) and was used to allocate the children to family social class groups using categories defined in the 1991 British Standard Occupational Classification system as follows: I, professional occupations; II, managerial and technical occupations; IIIN, skilled nonmanual occupations; IIIM, skilled manual occupations; IV, partly skilled occupations; V, unskilled occupations (10).

Information on parity and on maternal smoking in pregnancy was also obtained from questionnaire responses. Information on maternal smoking was grouped as follows: 1) never smoked; 2) smoked before pregnancy or in the first trimester and then stopped; 3) smoked throughout pregnancy.

Statistical analysis

Both the SEA scores and IQs were standardized (each value was divided by its standard deviation) to allow direct comparison. Multivariable logistic and linear regression models were used to assess the associations of GWG (using IOM categories and estimates from the multilevel model described above and in the Web Appendix) with offspring outcomes. Because of the nonrarity of the outcome we have not assumed that the resulting odds ratios are indicative of risk ratios but describe them throughout as odds ratios. Furthermore, we repeated these analyses by using Poisson regression. Analyses were conducted in STATA, version 11.0, software (StataCorp LP, College Station, Texas).

The IOM analysis included the following models. Model 1 was adjusted for gestational age, maternal age, age at outcome assessment, and sex. Model 2 was adjusted for the confounders listed for model 1 as well as prepregnancy BMI, maternal smoking during pregnancy, parity, mode of delivery, and maternal education. We consider model 2 to be the main model. We also explored potential mediation by birth weight to explore whether fetal growth is driving any association and by SEA scores (for IQ and final-examination results), both of which are reported in Web Tables 2 and 3.

Models in which GWG was expressed using estimates from the multilevel model were the same, except that we also adjusted for prepregnancy weight and weight gain occurring earlier in pregnancy. Moreover, in these models we did not adjust for gestational age as this is accounted for in the estimates from the multilevel models. We tested for possible interactions between being overweight/obese before pregnancy (indicated by a BMI of ≥25) and GWG in their association with outcomes.

RESULTS

Characteristics of women included or excluded from analyses because of missing confounder or outcome data are presented in Web Table 4. Although some statistical evidence of differences was found, the magnitude of these differences was generally small. Table 1 shows the characteristics of mothers and offspring according to whether the mothers gained less than, more than, or as recommended weight in pregnancy. Women who gained less than recommended were slightly older and were more likely to smoke in pregnancy, to be from a manual social class, and to be multiparous than women who gained as recommended. Women who gained less than recommended also had babies with a lower mean birth weight. Women who gained more than recommended were slightly younger, were less likely to smoke in pregnancy, and were less educated compared with women who gained as recommended. They were also more likely to be nulliparous, to have had a cesarean delivery, and to have had heavier offspring. Table 2 shows SEA scores at age 4 years and IQs at age 8 years by final-examination results at age 16 years. Children who achieved adequate final-examination results had higher mean SEA scores compared with children who did not.

Table 1.

Participant Characteristics by Institute of Medicine Category, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, Bristol, United Kingdom, 1990–Present

Less Than Recommended GWG (n = 2,946)
P ValueaAs Recommended GWG (n = 3,323)
More Than Recommended GWG (n = 2,384)
P Valuea
Mean (SD)%No.Mean (SD)%No.Mean (SD)%No.
Maternal age at birth, years29.0 (4.9)0.0528.7 (4.7)27.9 (4.6)<0.001
Did not smoke at all during pregnancy762,241<0.001812,678771,8410.001
Maternal education—no higher education832,4520.81842,807892,124<0.001
Manual social class195590.3516527184370.35
No previous pregnancies401,177<0.001471,573541,297<0.001
Cesarean delivery92670.2010 (333)13 (309)0.001
Prepregnancy weight, kg58.5 (11.8)<0.00159.6 (11.1)65.5 (13.1)<0.001
GWG, kg
 0–18 weeksb0.23 (0.15)<0.0010.32 (0.15)0.40 (0.19)<0.001
 18–28 weeksb0.42 (0.13)<0.0010.55 (0.13)0.68 (0.16)<0.001
 After 28 weeksb0.32 (0.14)<0.0010.48 (0.14)0.65 (0.18)<0.001
Offspring sex, males511,4920.69511,700511,2110.79
Gestational age, weeks39.6 (1.3)<0.00139.8 (1.3)39.9 (1.3)0.003
Birth weight, g3,333.4 (445.4)<0.0013,511.4 (445.2)3,639.6 (481.1)<0.001
SEA score13.0 (3.2)0.00413.3 (3.0)13.1 (3.2)0.098
IQ at age 8 years104.8 (17.1)0.22105.4 (16.0)105.0 (16.2)0.47
Adequate final-examination resultsc551,366591,652541,1090.001
Prepregnancy BMId categories
 <18.547.9488<0.00141.542210.6108< 0.001
 18.5–24.936.92,13340.92,36422.21281
 25–29.915.718531.437052.9622
 ≥3020.614024.616754.8680

Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; GWG, gestational weight gain; IQ, intelligence quotient; SD, standard deviation; SEA, School Entry Assessment.

a Compared with the category of “as recommended” GWG.

b GWG categories are based on knot points.

c “Adequate final-examination results” indicates a passing grade on 5 or more General Certificate of Secondary Education examinations, typically taken by students in the United Kingdom at age 16 years.

d BMI is measured and defined as weight (kg)/height (m)2.

Table 2.

Mean Values (Standard Deviation) of SEA Scores and IQ by Achievement of Adequate Final-Examination Results at Age 16 Years, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, Bristol, United Kingdom, 1990–Present

Adequate Final-Examination Resultsa
P Valueb
Yes (n = 3,212) (44%)
No (n = 4,127) (56%)
Mean (SD)No.Mean (SD)No.
SEA scores14.24 (2.75)2,92611.76 (3.01)2,431<0.001
IQ109.65 (14.03)3,56694.28 (15.31)2,172<0.001

Abbreviations: IQ, intelligence quotient; SD, standard deviation; SEA, School Entry Assessment.

a “Adequate final-examination results” indicates a passing grade on 5 or more General Certificate of Secondary Education examinations, typically taken by students in the United Kingdom at age 16 years.

b P value based on 2-tailed Student's t test.

Table 3 shows the multivariable associations of IOM categories of GWG with offspring cognitive outcomes. Offspring of women who gained less than recommended had lower mean SEA scores, even when adjusting for potential confounders (model 2). These children were also less likely to achieve adequate final-examination results at age 16 years in the basic and confounder-adjusted models (models 1 and 2). No association was found between less than recommended GWG and offspring IQ. Adjustment for birth weight did not attenuate the association with SEA scores but did slightly attenuate the relationship between GWG and final-examination results. Adjustment for SEA scores did not result in further attenuation (see Web Table 2).

Table 3.

Associations of Institute of Medicine Categories of Maternal GWG With Offspring Cognition, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, Bristol, United Kingdom, 1990–Present

OutcomeModelLess Than Recommended GWG
As Recommended GWG (Referent)More Than Recommended GWG
Mean SD DifferenceOdds Ratio95% CIMean SD DifferenceOdds Ratio95% CI
SEA scoresa (n = 5,832)1b−0.100−0.155, −0.0460−0.230−0.080, 0.033
2c−0.075−0.127, −0.02300.011−0.045, 0.067
IQ at age 8 yearsa (n = 5,191)1b−0.045−0.108, 0.01700.017−0.05, 0.083
2c−0.030−0.089, 0.02900.066−0.002, 0.131
Adequate final-examination resultsd (n = 7,339)1b0.840.75, 0.9410.890.79, 1.00
2c0.880.78, 0.9911.000.88, 1.14

Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; CI, confidence interval; GWG, gestational weight gain; IQ, intelligence quotient; SD, standard deviation; SEA, School Entry Assessment.

a Standardized. SEA, SD = 3.26; IQ, SD = 16.47.

b Model 1—adjusted for gestational age, maternal age, age at outcome assessment, and sex.

c Model 2—as model 1, plus additional adjustment for prepregnancy BMI (measured and defined as weight (kg)/height (m)2), parity, maternal smoking, maternal education, and mode of delivery (vaginal or cesarean).

d “Adequate final-examination results” indicates a passing grade on 5 or more General Certificate of Secondary Education examinations, typically taken by students in the United Kingdom at age 16 years.

There was no strong evidence for associations of more than recommended GWG with SEA scores or IQs. More than recommended GWG was associated with decreased odds of obtaining adequate final-examination results in the unadjusted model (model 1), but this attenuated to the null when adjusting for confounders. Results of the equivalent analyses for “high achievement” (9 or more GCSE grades of A* or A) and “low achievement” (0 passing GCSE grades) are presented in Web Table 5. There was no strong evidence of associations between IOM categories of GWG and high achievement, but both less than recommended GWG and more than recommended GWG were associated with greater odds of low GCSE achievement.

Table 4 shows the associations of prepregnancy weight and GWG with offspring cognitive outcomes by using the estimates from the multilevel model. There was no evidence of an interaction between prepregnancy overweight/obesity and GWG in relation to SEA scores and IQs (all P values for interaction > 0.14). There was evidence of an interaction between prepregnancy overweight/obesity and GWG in relation to final-examination results at age 16 years (all P values for interaction < 0.004), and we have presented results stratified by prepregnancy overweight/obesity for this outcome.

Table 4.

Multivariable Associations of Prepregnancy Weight and GWG With Offspring Cognition, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, Bristol, United Kingdom, 1990–Present

OutcomePrepregnancy Weight
GWG During Early Pregnancy (0–18 Weeksa)
GWG During Midpregnancy (18–28 Weeksa)
GWG During Late Pregnancy (28 Weeks and Latera)
Mean SD Difference per1 kgOdds Ratio95% CIMean SD Difference per 400 g/week95% CIMean SD Difference per 400 g/week95% CIMean SD Difference per 400 g/week95%CI
SEA scoresb (n = 5,832)
 Model 1c−0.006−0.008, −0.0040.1280.760, 0.1800.1670.114, 0.2200.1220.075, 0.170
 Model 2d−0.004−0.005, −0.0020.0720.019, 0.1240.0770.017, 0.1360.020−0.041, 0.081
IQ at age 8 yearsb (n = 5,191)
 Model 1c−0.006−0.009, −0.0040.1260.064, 0.1880.150.084, 0.2110.1430.087, 0.199
 Model 2d−0.004−0.006, −0.0020.0780.017, 0.1390.0720.004, 0.1390.0700.000, 0.139
BMIe<25BMIe≥25BMIe<25BMIe≥25BMIe<25BMIe≥25
OR
95% CI
OR
95% CI
OR
95% CI
OR
95% CI
OR
95% CI
OR
95% CI
Adequate final-  examination resultsf (n = 7,339)
 Model 1c0.980.98, 0.990.890.78, 1.021.551.27, 1.881.020.89, 1.161.601.32, 1.961.131.01, 1.271.591.33, 1.90
 Model 2d0.990.98, 0.990.890.77, 1.031.371.11, 1.701.030.87, 1.201.140.89, 1.471.000.85, 1.171.481.15, 1.92

Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; CI, confidence interval; GWG, gestational weight gain; IQ, intelligence quotient; OR, odds ratio; SD, standard deviation; SEA, School Entry Assessment.

a Gestational age categories based on knot points.

b Standardized. SEA, SD = 3.26; IQ, SD = 16.47.

c Model 1—adjusted for gestational age, maternal age, age at outcome assessment, and sex.

d Model 2—as model 2, plus additional adjustment for prepregnancy weight, GWG in previous period, parity, maternal smoking, maternal education, and mode of delivery (vaginal or cesarean).

e BMI is measured and defined as weight (kg)/height (m)2.

f “Adequate final-examination results” indicates a passing grade on 5 or more General Certificate of Secondary Education examinations, typically taken by students in the United Kingdom at age 16 years.

Prepregnancy weight was inversely associated with SEA scores, IQs, and the odds of achieving adequate final-examination results at age 16 years in model 1. These associations remained largely unchanged with adjustment for potential confounders (model 2). Because thinner women gain more during pregnancy (6), we also repeated the analysis for prepregnancy weight while adjusting for all GWG variables. Results (available upon request) were unchanged from those presented. Adjustment for birth weight did not importantly change the associations, but adjustment for SEA scores attenuated the association of prepregnancy weight with IQs and final-examination results (see Web Table 3 for all mediation analyses).

GWG in all 3 periods of pregnancy was positively associated with IQs in both confounder-adjusted models (models 1 and 2). GWG in all 3 periods was positively associated with SEA scores in model 1. The associations of early pregnancy and midpregnancy GWG with SEA scores persisted in the fully adjusted model (model 2) and were of similar magnitude to associations with IQs. The association of later pregnancy GWG with SEA scores was attenuated toward the null in the fully adjusted confounder model (model 2). Adjustment for birth weight slightly attenuated models for SEA scores and IQs (Web Table 3).

In all models, GWG in early pregnancy was negatively associated with obtaining adequate final-examination results at age 16 years for offspring of women who were not overweight/obese prepregnancy, though confidence intervals included the null value. In contrast, in overweight women, GWG in early pregnancy was positively associated with the odds of achieving adequate final-examination results in all models. In midpregnancy, there was no evidence of an association between GWG and final-examination results in offspring of women with a normal BMI (18.5–24.9), but in overweight/obese women there was a strong positive association in model 1, which attenuated when adjusted for confounders (model 2). In late pregnancy, in women who were not overweight/obese, there was a positive association in model 1, which attenuated to the null when confounders (model 2) were included in the models. A strong positive association between GWG and final-examination results was found in overweight/obese women and remained when adjusting for confounders (model 2). Mediator adjustment did not attenuate the relationship in early or late pregnancy. In midpregnancy, birth weight further attenuated the relationship, but SEA scores did not.

When analyses were restricted to mother-offspring pairs who had all outcomes measured (n = 3,340), similar results to those presented here were obtained, although confidence intervals increased in width as expected (see Web Tables 6 and 7). When analyses were restricted to women without diabetes in pregnancy (existing or gestational diabetes, n = 36 excluded), point estimates for SEA scores and final-examination results were comparable to those presented. However, for IQs, point estimates became more pronounced. For example, in IOM model 3 (adjusting for confounders plus birth weight), the point estimate increased from 0.066 to 0.205 for those who gained more than recommended. Replacing maternal education with household social class in the analyses did not change results. Finally, when analyses for final-examination results were repeated by using Poisson regression (Web Tables 8 and 9), results were in the same direction as those presented here but point estimates were closer to the null value. This is as expected because odds ratios will overestimate risk when an outcome is not rare.

DISCUSSION

In this contemporary birth cohort, we found that women who gained less weight during pregnancy than recommended by the 2009 IOM guidelines had offspring who achieved lower SEA scores at age 4 years and who were less likely to achieve adequate final-examination results at age 16 years, even when adjusting for potential confounders including maternal education. Offspring of women who gained more than recommended also had poorer final-examination results at age 16 years. Examining the associations in more detail revealed that higher prepregnancy weight was associated with poorer outcomes for SEA scores at age 4 years, IQ at age 8 years, and final-examination results at age 16 years, and that greater GWG throughout pregnancy was generally associated with better cognitive outcomes in offspring. Birth weight seemed to partially mediate associations between GWG and outcomes, as did the association of GWG with SEA scores in relation to later outcomes (IQs and final-examination results). Associations in most cases were weakened but not completely attenuated. The magnitudes of the associations that we have observed are generally modest in size and are unlikely to have major importance for individuals. However, at a population level modest shifts in the distribution of educational attainment could be important. Any potential benefit to IQ and educational attainment that might be achieved from increasing GWG would need to be considered alongside possible adverse effects on offspring adiposity and cardiovascular risk.

Results of our analysis using IOM categories of GWG are broadly consistent with those from a previous study (n = 2,590) that found cognitive scores to be higher in offspring of mothers who gained between 2 and 13 kg during gestation than in those whose mothers gained less than 2 kg or more than 13 kg (7). Our study is considerably larger, and we were able to examine associations in greater detail with repeated measurements of weight during pregnancy.

GWG is a complex measure that includes maternal fat deposition, volume expansion, placenta, amniotic fluid, and fetus. In terms of plausible mechanisms by which GWG might influence the cognitive and educational outcomes of offspring, several of these components might be involved. Greater maternal fat deposition might result in greater delivery of glucose and fatty acids to the developing fetus, which in turn might influence fetal brain development. Fetal size may reflect particular environmental exposures affecting growth in general and brain development in particular. We were able to examine some of these in our more detailed analysis. We did not find clear evidence that GWG in the first 18 weeks of pregnancy, when the contribution of maternal fat deposition to overall GWG is greatest, is specifically associated with cognitive outcomes, suggesting that maternal fat deposition may not be the main driver of associations. In our study, birth weight partially mediated associations of GWG with offspring cognitive outcomes, suggesting that fetal growth is not the main driver of associations. However, adjusting for birth weight is not simple, because unless all confounders of the relationship are included, conditioning on a mediator can result in collider bias (11, 12). The analysis adjusting for birth weight (Web Tables 2 and 3) should be interpreted bearing this in mind (13).

Our findings may reflect the association of fetal growth with childhood cognition. Alternatively, the observed association of birth weight with IQ and, here of GWG with cognitive outcomes could be attributable to residual confounding and not reflections of a direct intrauterine effect (14). In particular, it may be that problems during pregnancy such as preeclampsia could result in changes in GWG, rendering it not a completely modifiable risk factor, as placental problems during pregnancy would cause changes in both GWG and offspring cognition. However, even if this is the case and the association is a product of such residual confounding and not causal, monitoring of GWG may still be important.

Our finding that prepregnancy weight is inversely associated with offspring cognition is hard to explain given the postulated mechanism suggesting that adequate supply of nourishment is necessary for healthy fetal brain development. According to this mechanism we would expect both prepregnancy weight and GWG, particularly in early pregnancy, to be positively associated with offspring cognition across their distribution in a general, healthy population. One possibility is that smoking and/or socioeconomic position confounds the association of prepregnancy weight with offspring cognition; however, we adjusted for both of these factors in our analysis. Another possibility is that weight acquisition in pregnancy is somehow different from prepregnancy weight in relation to offspring IQ (though not to other outcomes) (8, 15). Finally, because thinner women gain more in pregnancy (6) our estimate for prepregnancy weight could be biased. To test this possibility, we repeated the analysis for prepregnancy weight while adjusting for GWG in all periods. Results were unchanged from those presented (results available on request).

Study strengths and limitations

To our knowledge, this is the first general population study with repeat measurements of weight in pregnancy to examine the association of GWG with offspring cognition, an area of research that was highlighted as important in the IOM report (6). We were able to examine cognitive outcomes across childhood from age 4–16 years, including educational attainment, which has an important impact on future health and wellbeing. Our sample size is relatively large and we were able to adjust for a wide range of potential confounding factors, including maternal education, though because our study is observational, residual confounding cannot be ruled out. Replicating these findings in additional studies with similar detailed measurements of gestational weight would be valuable. Sibling studies, which inherently control for shared genetic and environmental factors and have been used previously to examine GWG in relation to offspring BMI in later life (16), may be useful to explore GWG's association with offspring cognition.

Not all mother-offspring pairs were included in analyses because of missing data, mostly outcome data. However, this should bias the results only if missingness depends on the outcome, conditional on all the variables in the model. The general similarity of findings across the 3 outcomes (which were available on slightly different groups) suggests that our associations are not markedly biased by missing data.

Conclusion

Prepregnancy weight is inversely associated with childhood IQ and school achievement; however, GWG is positively associated with outcomes, even when controlling for available potential confounders. Although previous findings based on the same cohort suggest that less than recommended GWG (by 2009 IOM guidelines) is associated with a healthier cardiovascular profile in both mothers (15) and offspring (8), this study emphasizes the need to recognize that defining optimal GWG is complex with competing risks at play.

Supplementary Material

Web Material:

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Author affiliations: Centre for Causal Analyses in Translational Epidemiology, School of Social and Community Medicine, Bristol, United Kingdom (Suzanne H. Gage, Debbie A. Lawlor, Abigail Fraser); and School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom (Kate Tilling).

The work presented in this paper was funded by grants from the Wellcome Trust (grant 087997) and the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, National Institutes of Health (grant R01 DK077659). A.F. is funded by a United Kingdom Medical Research Council research fellowship (grant 0701594). The United Kingdom Medical Research Council (grant 74882), the Wellcome Trust (grant 076467), and the University of Bristol provide core funding support for ALSPAC. The United Kingdom Medical Research Council and the University of Bristol provide core funding for the Medical Research Council Centre for Causal Analyses in Translational Epidemiology. The latter funds S.G.'s studentship (grant SD1708).

The views expressed in this paper are those of the authors and not necessarily those of any funding body or others whose support is acknowledged. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest: none declared.

REFERENCES

1. Rizzo T, Metzger BE, Burns WJ, et al. Correlations between antepartum maternal metabolism and child intelligence. N Engl J Med. 1991;325(13):911–916. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
2. Silverman BL, Rizzo T, Green OC, et al. Long-term prospective evaluation of offspring of diabetic mothers. Diabetes. 1991;40(2):121–125. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
3. Stehbens JA, Baker GL, Kitchell M. Outcome at ages 1, 3, and 5 years of children born to diabetic women. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1977;127(4):408–413. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
4. Stein Z, Susser M, Saenger G, et al. Nutrition and mental performance. Science. 1972;178(62):708–713. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
5. de Rooij SR, Wouters H, Yonker JE, et al. Prenatal undernutrition and cognitive function in late adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(39):16881–16886. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
6. Rasmussen KM, Yaktine AL. Weight Gain During Pregnancy: Reexamining the Guidelines. Washington, DC: Institute of Medicine; 2009. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
7. Tavris DR, Read JA. Effect of maternal weight gain on fetal, infant, and childhood death and on cognitive development. Obstet Gynecol. 1982;60(6):689–694. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
8. Fraser A, Tilling K, Macdonald-Wallis C, et al. Association of maternal weight gain in pregnancy with offspring obesity and metabolic and vascular traits in childhood. Circulation. 2010;121(23):2557–2564. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
9. Wechsler D, Golombok J, Rust J. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Sidcup, United Kingdom: The Psychological Corporation; 1992. [Google Scholar]
10. Office of Population Censuses and Surveys. Standard Occupational Classification. London, England: Her Majesty's Stationery Office; 1991. [Google Scholar]
11. Hernandez-Diaz S, Schisterman EF, Hernan MA. The birth weight "paradox" uncovered? Am J Epidemiol. 2006;164(11):1115–1120. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
12. Hernan MA, Hernandez-Diaz S, Werler MM, et al. Causal knowledge as a prerequisite for confounding evaluation: an application to birth defects epidemiology. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;155(2):176–184. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
13. Adams M, Andersen AM, Andersen PK, et al. Sostrup statement on low birthweight. Int J Epidemiol. 2003;32(5):884–885. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
14. Yang S, Lynch J, Susser ES, et al. Birth weight and cognitive ability in childhood among siblings and nonsiblings. Pediatrics. 2008;122(2):e350–e358. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
15. Fraser A, Tilling K, Macdonald-Wallis C, et al. Associations of gestational weight gain with maternal body mass index, waist circumference, and blood pressure measured 16 y after pregnancy: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;93(6):1285–1292. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
16. Lawlor DA, Lichtenstein P, Fraser A, et al. Does maternal weight gain in pregnancy have long-term effects on offspring adiposity? A sibling study in a prospective cohort of 146,894 men from 136,050 families. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;94(1):142–148. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]

Articles from American Journal of Epidemiology are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

Citations & impact 


Impact metrics

Jump to Citations

Citations of article over time

Alternative metrics

Altmetric item for https://www.altmetric.com/details/1224452
Altmetric
Discover the attention surrounding your research
https://www.altmetric.com/details/1224452

Article citations


Go to all (32) article citations

Data 


Data behind the article

This data has been text mined from the article, or deposited into data resources.

Similar Articles 


To arrive at the top five similar articles we use a word-weighted algorithm to compare words from the Title and Abstract of each citation.


Funding 


Funders who supported this work.

Economic and Social Research Council (1)

Medical Research Council (5)

NIDDK NIH HHS (1)